Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life

This article presents empirical evidence of variability and change in the performance of young children in causality tasks, highlighting the relevance of nonlinear perspective in studies about the first year of life. Using a longitudinal (12 weeks of observation) and cross-sectional (four age groups...

Full description

Autores:
Tipo de recurso:
Fecha de publicación:
2013
Institución:
Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar
Repositorio:
Repositorio Institucional UTB
Idioma:
eng
OAI Identifier:
oai:repositorio.utb.edu.co:20.500.12585/9083
Acceso en línea:
https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12585/9083
Palabra clave:
Cognitive change
Cognitive development
Operative causality
Uzgiris Hunt Scale
Variability
Rights
restrictedAccess
License
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
id UTB2_54f65b0d9c28f9842abb36d704114569
oai_identifier_str oai:repositorio.utb.edu.co:20.500.12585/9083
network_acronym_str UTB2
network_name_str Repositorio Institucional UTB
repository_id_str
dc.title.none.fl_str_mv Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
dc.title.alternative.none.fl_str_mv Cambio y variabilidad: Un marco de referencia en los estudios sobre el primer año de vida
title Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
spellingShingle Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
Cognitive change
Cognitive development
Operative causality
Uzgiris Hunt Scale
Variability
title_short Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
title_full Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
title_fullStr Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
title_full_unstemmed Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
title_sort Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of life
dc.subject.keywords.none.fl_str_mv Cognitive change
Cognitive development
Operative causality
Uzgiris Hunt Scale
Variability
topic Cognitive change
Cognitive development
Operative causality
Uzgiris Hunt Scale
Variability
description This article presents empirical evidence of variability and change in the performance of young children in causality tasks, highlighting the relevance of nonlinear perspective in studies about the first year of life. Using a longitudinal (12 weeks of observation) and cross-sectional (four age groups) design, the strategy trajectories used by110 children in solving the tasks adapted from the Operatory Causality Subscale of the Uzgiris- Hunt Functioning Scale (Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975) are described. For data treatment and analysis mixed methods were employed. From the qualitative perspective, the Situation Analysis Method was used to identify the cognitive demands that situations require. Subsequently, by means of a microgenetic analysis, the action programs of children were classified into three types of strategies: (1) Non Resolution, (2) Exploration, and (3) Resolution. Additionally, knowledge formats underlying these strategies were established. From a quantitative perspective, the strategy trajectories were analyzed by clusters (k-means).The clusters obtained with this analysis allowed to quantify the kind of strategies used by children in each resolution event of the Operative Causality Subscale. Results show: (a) the variability of strategies in the performance trajectories that make up each cluster, and (b) the change of strategies (e.g. from exploration to resolution) as an adaptive developmental resource. Finally, a case study is presented to illustrate the cognitive shift through the transition from implicit knowledge to explicit knowledge in a recurring phase model.
publishDate 2013
dc.date.issued.none.fl_str_mv 2013
dc.date.accessioned.none.fl_str_mv 2020-03-26T16:32:54Z
dc.date.available.none.fl_str_mv 2020-03-26T16:32:54Z
dc.type.coarversion.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
dc.type.coar.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.type.driver.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.hasVersion.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
dc.type.spa.none.fl_str_mv Artículo
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.citation.none.fl_str_mv Acta Colombiana de Psicologia; Vol. 16, Núm. 1; pp. 101-113
dc.identifier.issn.none.fl_str_mv 01239155
dc.identifier.uri.none.fl_str_mv https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12585/9083
dc.identifier.instname.none.fl_str_mv Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar
dc.identifier.reponame.none.fl_str_mv Repositorio UTB
dc.identifier.orcid.none.fl_str_mv 56185436000
24461215800
56185476200
identifier_str_mv Acta Colombiana de Psicologia; Vol. 16, Núm. 1; pp. 101-113
01239155
Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar
Repositorio UTB
56185436000
24461215800
56185476200
url https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12585/9083
dc.language.iso.none.fl_str_mv eng
language eng
dc.rights.coar.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_16ec
dc.rights.uri.none.fl_str_mv http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.rights.accessRights.none.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/restrictedAccess
dc.rights.cc.none.fl_str_mv Atribución-NoComercial 4.0 Internacional
rights_invalid_str_mv http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
Atribución-NoComercial 4.0 Internacional
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_16ec
eu_rights_str_mv restrictedAccess
dc.format.medium.none.fl_str_mv Recurso electrónico
dc.format.mimetype.none.fl_str_mv application/pdf
dc.publisher.none.fl_str_mv Universidad Catolica de Colombia
publisher.none.fl_str_mv Universidad Catolica de Colombia
dc.source.none.fl_str_mv https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-84901669382&partnerID=40&md5=b38a5e414dea12b26cb1dde9f8eedf54
institution Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar
bitstream.url.fl_str_mv https://repositorio.utb.edu.co/bitstream/20.500.12585/9083/1/MiniProdInv.png
bitstream.checksum.fl_str_mv 0cb0f101a8d16897fb46fc914d3d7043
bitstream.checksumAlgorithm.fl_str_mv MD5
repository.name.fl_str_mv Repositorio Institucional UTB
repository.mail.fl_str_mv repositorioutb@utb.edu.co
_version_ 1814021788283174912
spelling 2020-03-26T16:32:54Z2020-03-26T16:32:54Z2013Acta Colombiana de Psicologia; Vol. 16, Núm. 1; pp. 101-11301239155https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12585/9083Universidad Tecnológica de BolívarRepositorio UTB561854360002446121580056185476200This article presents empirical evidence of variability and change in the performance of young children in causality tasks, highlighting the relevance of nonlinear perspective in studies about the first year of life. Using a longitudinal (12 weeks of observation) and cross-sectional (four age groups) design, the strategy trajectories used by110 children in solving the tasks adapted from the Operatory Causality Subscale of the Uzgiris- Hunt Functioning Scale (Uzgiris & Hunt, 1975) are described. For data treatment and analysis mixed methods were employed. From the qualitative perspective, the Situation Analysis Method was used to identify the cognitive demands that situations require. Subsequently, by means of a microgenetic analysis, the action programs of children were classified into three types of strategies: (1) Non Resolution, (2) Exploration, and (3) Resolution. Additionally, knowledge formats underlying these strategies were established. From a quantitative perspective, the strategy trajectories were analyzed by clusters (k-means).The clusters obtained with this analysis allowed to quantify the kind of strategies used by children in each resolution event of the Operative Causality Subscale. Results show: (a) the variability of strategies in the performance trajectories that make up each cluster, and (b) the change of strategies (e.g. from exploration to resolution) as an adaptive developmental resource. Finally, a case study is presented to illustrate the cognitive shift through the transition from implicit knowledge to explicit knowledge in a recurring phase model.Recurso electrónicoapplication/pdfengUniversidad Catolica de Colombiahttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/info:eu-repo/semantics/restrictedAccessAtribución-NoComercial 4.0 Internacionalhttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_16echttps://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-84901669382&partnerID=40&md5=b38a5e414dea12b26cb1dde9f8eedf54Change and variability: A framework in studies on the first year of lifeCambio y variabilidad: Un marco de referencia en los estudios sobre el primer año de vidainfo:eu-repo/semantics/articleinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionArtículohttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1Cognitive changeCognitive developmentOperative causalityUzgiris Hunt ScaleVariabilityRíos H.S.Ceballos E.C.Guerrero M.G.Baillargeon, R., De Vos, J., Object permanence in 3,5 and 4,5 months old infants: Further evidence (1991) Child Development, 62, pp. 1227-1246Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, T., Camaioni, L., Volterra, V., (1979) The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy, , N.Y.: Academic PressBower, T., (1984) El mundo Perceptivo del Niño, , Madrid: MorataBruner, J., From communication to language: A psychological perspective (1975) Cognition, 3, pp. 255-328Cohen, L.B., Oakes, L.M., How infants perceive a simple causal event (1993) Developmental Psychology, 29, pp. 421-433Courage, M., Howe, M., From infant to child. The dynamics of cognitive change in the second year of life (2002) Psychological Bulletin, 128 (2), pp. 250-277Fischer, K., Bidell, T., Developmental of psychological structures in action and thought (1998) Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development, pp. 467-561. , En W. Damon and R.M. Lerner (Eds), N.Y.: John Wiley and SonsFlynn, E., Siegler, R., Measuring Change: Current trends and future directions in microgenetic research (2007) Infant and Child Development, 16, pp. 135-149Fodor, J., (1983) La Modularidad de la mente, , Barcelona: GedisaKarmiloff-Smith, A., (1994) Más allá de la Modularidad. La Ciencia Cognitiva desde la Perspectiva del Desarrollo., , Madrid: Alianza EditorialMandler, J., How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives (1992) Psychological Review, 99, pp. 587-604Mandler, J., Representation (1998) Handbook of Child Psychology. Cognition, Perception, and Language, 2, pp. 255-308. , Fifth edition, En D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), N.Y.: WileyMeltzoff, A.N., Moore, M.K., Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures (1983) Child Development, 54, pp. 702-709Miller, P.H., Order in variability, variability in order: Why it matters for theories of development (2002) Human Development, 45, pp. 161-166Miller, P.H., Coyle, T.R., Developmental change: Lessons from microgenesis (1999) Conceptual Development: Piaget's Legacy, pp. 209-239. , En E.K. Scholnick, K. Nelson, S.A. Gelman & P.H. Miller (Eds.), Mahwah, N. J: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesOrozco, M., Ochoa, S., Sanchez, H., (2001) Prácticas Culturales y Educación de la Niñez, , Bogotá: Fundación Antonio Restrepo BarcoOrozco, M., Perinat, A., Sánchez, H., Cognitive Development and Interaction Context (2009) Living in Poverty: Developmental Poetics of Cultural Realities, pp. 287-308. , En A. C. Bastos & E. Prabinovich (Eds.), Charlott, NC: Information Age PublishingPascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., The psychological unit and its role in task analysis: A reinterpretation of object permanence (1991) Criteria for Competence: Controversies in the Conceptualization and Assessment of Children's Abilities, pp. 151-187. , En M. Chandler, & M. Chapman (Eds.), Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Earlbaun AssociatesPerner, J., (1994) Comprender la Mente Representacional., , Barcelona: PaidosPiaget, J., (1985) La Toma de Conciencia, , Madrid: MorataPiaget, J., (1989) La Construcción de lo Real en el Niño, , BarcelonaCrítica, S.A., Piaget, J., (1990) El Nacimiento de la Inteligencia en el niño, , Barcelona: Editorial CríticaPuche-Navarro, R., (2000) Formación de herramientas científicas en el niño pequeño, , Santafé de Bogotá: Arango EditoresPuche-Navarro, R., (2003) El niño que Piensa y Vuelve a Pensar, , Cali: Artes Gráficas del Valle EditoresPuche-Navarro, R., Érase una vez el desarrollo (2008) Claves para pensar el cambio: Ensayo sobre psicología del desarrollo, pp. 27-69. , En F. Larreamendy-Joerns, R. Puche & A. Restrepo (Eds.), Bogotá: Uniandes-CESOSánchez, H., Guevara, M., Cerchiaro, E., Desarrollo y/o cambio de la noción de objeto permanente y causalidad operatoria: Evidencia empírica en el primer año de vida, , (en prensa), Aceptado para publicación en la revista Avances en Psicología LatinoamericanaSiegler, R.S., Crowley, K., The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development (1991) American Psychologist, 46, pp. 606-620Siegler, R.S., Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development (1994) Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3 (1), pp. 1-5Siegler, R., (1996) Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking, , N.Y.: Oxford University PressSiegler, R., Variability and infant development (2002) Infant Behavior & Development, 25, pp. 550-557Siegler, R., Microgenetic analyses of learning (2006) Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language, pp. 464-510. , En W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, D. Kuhn, and R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Hoboken, NJ: WileyUzgiris, Y., Hunt, J., (1975) Assessment in infancy: Ordinal Scales of psychological development., , Chicago: University of Illinois PressYan, Z., Fischer, K., Always under construction: Dynamic variations in adult cognitive microdevelopment (2002) Human Development, 45, pp. 141-160http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501THUMBNAILMiniProdInv.pngMiniProdInv.pngimage/png23941https://repositorio.utb.edu.co/bitstream/20.500.12585/9083/1/MiniProdInv.png0cb0f101a8d16897fb46fc914d3d7043MD5120.500.12585/9083oai:repositorio.utb.edu.co:20.500.12585/90832021-02-02 14:19:27.652Repositorio Institucional UTBrepositorioutb@utb.edu.co