Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios

La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el co...

Full description

Autores:
Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcántara, Vicenta
Tipo de recurso:
Article of journal
Fecha de publicación:
2019
Institución:
Universidad Católica de Colombia
Repositorio:
RIUCaC - Repositorio U. Católica
Idioma:
spa
OAI Identifier:
oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/23661
Acceso en línea:
https://hdl.handle.net/10983/23661
Palabra clave:
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELACIONES INTERPERSONALES
COGNICIÓN
COGNITIVE RESERVE
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS
COGNITION
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELAÇÕES INTERPESSOAIS
COGNIÇÃO
Rights
openAccess
License
Derechos Reservados - Universidad Católica de Colombia, 2019
id UCATOLICA2_aded4eb16b2ef0f8f4b0b90aa75852c4
oai_identifier_str oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/23661
network_acronym_str UCATOLICA2
network_name_str RIUCaC - Repositorio U. Católica
repository_id_str
dc.title.spa.fl_str_mv Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
dc.title.translated.eng.fl_str_mv Social engagement as a measurement of cognitive reserve and how it relates to different cognitive skills in college students
Compromisso social como medida de reserva cognitiva e sua relação com diferentes processos cognitivos em jovens universitários
title Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
spellingShingle Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELACIONES INTERPERSONALES
COGNICIÓN
COGNITIVE RESERVE
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS
COGNITION
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELAÇÕES INTERPESSOAIS
COGNIÇÃO
title_short Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
title_full Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
title_fullStr Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
title_full_unstemmed Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
title_sort Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios
dc.creator.fl_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcántara, Vicenta
dc.contributor.author.spa.fl_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcántara, Vicenta
dc.subject.proposal.spa.fl_str_mv RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELACIONES INTERPERSONALES
COGNICIÓN
COGNITIVE RESERVE
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS
COGNITION
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELAÇÕES INTERPESSOAIS
COGNIÇÃO
topic RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELACIONES INTERPERSONALES
COGNICIÓN
COGNITIVE RESERVE
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS
COGNITION
RESERVA COGNITIVA
RELAÇÕES INTERPESSOAIS
COGNIÇÃO
description La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el compromiso social (en sus dimensiones de contacto, apoyo y conflicto social) y algunos procesos cognitivos (como flexibilidad mental, planeación, fluidez verbal, velocidad de procesamiento y comprensión verbal) en jóvenes universitarios. Para esto, se realizó un estudio correlacional con un diseño transversal en una muestra de 49 estudiantes. El análisis de correlación de Spearman reveló correlaciones entre contacto social y fluidez verbal (r = -.299, p = .037), apoyo social y planeación (r = .368, p = .009), y conflicto social y velocidad de procesamiento (r = .306, p = .032). Estos resultados perfilan una asociación entre el compromiso social y algunas funciones cognitivas. A pesar de que los resultados no son del todo concluyentes, queda claro que se necesita de un estudio más detallado de estos procesos.
publishDate 2019
dc.date.accessioned.spa.fl_str_mv 2019-09-04T14:55:52Z
dc.date.available.spa.fl_str_mv 2019-09-04T14:55:52Z
dc.date.issued.spa.fl_str_mv 2019-12
dc.type.spa.fl_str_mv Artículo de revista
dc.type.coar.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.type.coarversion.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
dc.type.coar.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
dc.type.content.spa.fl_str_mv Text
dc.type.driver.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.redcol.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ART
dc.type.version.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
format http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.citation.spa.fl_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, E., & Reynoso-Alcantara, V. (2019). Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 22(2), 218-229. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
dc.identifier.issn.spa.fl_str_mv 0123-9155
dc.identifier.uri.spa.fl_str_mv https://hdl.handle.net/10983/23661
identifier_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, E., & Reynoso-Alcantara, V. (2019). Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 22(2), 218-229. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
0123-9155
url https://hdl.handle.net/10983/23661
dc.language.iso.spa.fl_str_mv spa
language spa
dc.relation.ispartof.spa.fl_str_mv Acta Colombiana de Psicología, Vol. 22 no. 2 (jul.-dic. 2019); p. 218-240
dc.relation.references.spa.fl_str_mv Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056.
Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Wilson, R. S., Bienias, J. L., & Evans, D. A. (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63(12), 2322-2326. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147473.04043.B3.
Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social Disengagement and Incident Cognitive Decline in Community-Dwelling Elderly Persons. Annals of Internal Medicine, 131(3), 165-173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002.
Bennett, D. A., Arnold, S. E., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C. & Schneider, J. A. (2014). Cognitive and social lifestyle: Links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathological; 127(1), 137-150. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2.
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., & Wilson, R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer's disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406-412. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70417-3.
Berkman, L. F. (2000). Which influences cognitive function: Living alone or being alone? The Lancet, 355(9212), 1291-1292. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02107-3.
Cornwell, B. & Laumann, E. O. (2015). The health benefits of network growth: New evidence from a national survey of older adults. Social Science Medicine; 125, 94-106. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.011.
Cremonte, M., Ledesma, R., Cherpitel, C., & Borges, G. (2010). Psychometric properties of alcohol screening tests in the emergency department in Argentina, Mexico and the United States. Addictive Behaviors, 35(9), 818-25. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.03.021.
Flores, J. C., Ostrosky, F., & Lozano, A. (2014). BANFE-2, Batería neurosicológica de funciones ejecutivas y lóbulos frontales [BANFE-2, Neurosychological battery of executive functions and frontal lobes]. México: Manual Moderno.
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology , 3(6), 343-353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7.
Gardner, P. (2014). The role of social engagement and identity in community mobility among older adults aging in place. Disability and Rehabilitation, 36(15), 1249-1257. doi:10.3109/09638288.2013.837970.
Gariépy, G., Honkaniemi, H., & Quesnel-Vallée, A. (2016). Social support and protection from depression: systematic review of current findings in Western countries. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 284-293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.169094.z.
Grossetti, M. (2009). ¿Qué es una relación social? Un conjunto de mediaciones diádicas [What is a social relation? A set of dyadic mediations]. REDES-Revista Hispana Para El Análisis de Redes Sociales, 6(2), 44-62. doi: 10.5565/REV/REDES.364.
Hernández, R., Fernández, C., & Baptista, P. (2010). Metodología de la investigación (5a ed.) [Investigation Methodology]. México: Mcgraw-Hill Educación.
Holtzman, R. E., Rebok, G. W., Saczynski, J. S., Kouzis, A. C., Wilcox Doyle, K., & Eaton, W. W. (2004). Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59(6), 278-284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P278.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472-503. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.4.472.
Krueger, K., & Wilson, R. (2009). Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1080/03610730802545028.SOCIAL.
La Fleur, C. G., & Salthouse, T. A. (2017). Which aspects of social support are associated with which cognitive abilities for which people? The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 72(6), 1006-1016. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv119.
Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Bigler, E., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Liao, J. & Scholes, S. (2017). Association of social support and cognitive aging modified by sex and relationship type: A prospective investigation in the English longitudinal study of ageing. American Journal of Epidemiology; 186(7), 787-795. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx142.
Liao, J., Head, J., Kumari, M., Stansfeld, S., Kivimaki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., & Brunner, E. J. (2014). Negative aspects of close relationships as risk factors for cognitive aging. American Journal of Epidemi; 180(11), 1118-1125. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu236.
Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., & Pereiro, A. X. (2014). El nivel de vocabulario como indicador de reserva cognitiva en la evaluación del deterioro cognitivo ligero [The level of vocabulary as an indicator of cognitive reserve in the evaluation of mild cognitive impairment]. Anales de Psicología, 30, 1115-1121. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.158481.
Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K., Rorden, C., Pickrd, J., & Robertson, I. H. (2003). Enhancing the Sensitivity of a Sustained Attention Task to Frontal Damage: Convergent Clinical and Functional Imaging Evidence. Neurocase, 9(4), 340-349. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.4.340.15553.
Marioni, R. E., Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Dartigues, J.-F., & Jacqmin-Gadda, H. (2015). Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: a 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health, 15(1), 1089. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2426-6.
Mayor, J., Amador, F., & Ramírez, I. (2008). La reserva cognitiva mejora la velocidad de procesamiento de los componentes centrales del tiempo de reacción en adultos mayores pero no en jóvenes [Cognitive reserve improves the processing speed of the central components of reaction time in older adults but not in young people]. Revista Cubana de Salud Y Trabajo, 9(1), 7-18.
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Review, 87(3), 873-904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006.
Nelson, L. A., Noonan, C. J., Goldberg, J., & Buchwald, D. S. (2013). Social Engagement and Physical and Cognitive Health Among American Indian Participants in the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 28(4), 453-463. doi: 10.1007/s10823-013-9213-6.
Nithianantharajah, J., & Hannan, A. J. (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 89(4), 369-382. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.001.
Pillemer, S. C., & Holtzer, R. (2016). The differential relationships of dimensions of perceived social support with cognitive function among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 20(7), 727-735. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1033683.
Rodriguez-Laso, A., Zunzunegui, M. V., & Otero, A. (2007). The effect of social relationships on survival in elderly residents of a Southern European community: A cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 7, 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-7-19.
Rosselli, M., Jurado, M. B., & Matute, E. (2008). Las Funciones Ejecutivas a través de la Vida [Executive Functions through Life]. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría Y Neurociencias, 8(1), 23-46. Retrieved from http://neurociencias.udea.edu.co/revista/PDF/REVNEURO_vol8_num1_5.pdf.
Saczynski, J. S. (2006). The Effect of Social Engagement on Incident Dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemi ology , 163(5), 433-440. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj061.
Scarmeas, N., & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section A), 25(5), 625-633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576.
Seeman, T., Lusignolo, T., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychology, 20(4), 243-255. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.243.
Seeman, T., Miller-Martinez, D., Stein Merkin, S., Lachman, M., Tun, P., & Karlamangla, A. (2011). Histories of Social Engagement and Adult Cognition: Midlife in the U.S. Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B (S1), i141-i152. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq091.
Segel-Karpas, D. & Lachman, M. E. (2016). Social contact and cognitive functioning: The role of personality. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences , 73(6), 974-984. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw079.
Skakkebœk, A., Moore, P.J., Pedersen, A.D., Bojesen, A., Kristensen, M. K., Fedder, J., Laurberg, P., Hertz, J. M., 0stergaard, J. R., Wallentin, M. & Gravholt, C. H. (2017). The role of genes, intelligence, personality, and social engagement in cognitive performance in Klinefelter syndrome. Brain and Behavior, 7(3), e00645. doi: 10.1002/brb3.645.
Soto-Añari, M., Flores-Valdivia, G., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2013). Nivel de lectura como medida de reserva cognitiva en adultos mayores [Reading level as a measure of cognitive reserve in older adults]. Revista de Neurologia, 56(2), 79-85.
SPSS Inc. Edición 2007. SPSS para Windows, Version 16.0. Chicago, SPSS Inc.
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004.
Tun, P. A., Miller-Martinez, D., Lachman, M. E., & Seeman, T. (2013). Social strain and executive function across the lifespan: The dark (and light) sides of social engagement. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 20(3), 320-338. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.707173.
Umberson, D., & Karas, J. (2010). Social Relationships and Health: A Flashpoint for Health Policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(S1), S54-S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501.
Whechsler, D. (2012). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV, WAIS-IV [Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV, WAIS-IV]. Manual de aplicación. Mexico, D.F.: Manual Moderno.
Wilson, R. S., Boyle P. A., James, B. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Negative social interactions and risk of mild cognitive impairment in old age. Neuropsychology; 29(4), 561-570. doi: 10.1037/neu0000154.
Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., Tang, Y, & Bennett, D. A. (2007). Loneliness and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(2), 234-240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234.
Zainab, N. & Naz, H. (2017). Daily living functioning, social engagement and wellness of older adults. Psychology, Community & Health, 6(1), 93-102. doi: 10.5964/pch.v6i1.213.
Zhou, Z., Wang, P., & Fang, Y. (2018). Social Engagement and Its Change are Associated with Dementia Risk among Chinese Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1551. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17879-w.
Zuelsdorff, M. L., Koscik, R. L., Okonkwo, O. C., Peppard, P. E., Hermann, B. P., Sager, M. A., Johnson, S. C. & Engelman, C. D. (2018). Reliability of a novel social activity questionnaire: Perceived social support and verbal interaction in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention. Journal of Aging and Health; 30(2), 305-320. doi: 10.1177/0898264316674812.
Zunzunegui, M.-V., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences , 58(2), S93-S100. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S93.
dc.rights.spa.fl_str_mv Derechos Reservados - Universidad Católica de Colombia, 2019
dc.rights.coar.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
dc.rights.accessrights.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rights.creativecommons.spa.fl_str_mv Atribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacional (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
dc.rights.uri.spa.fl_str_mv https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
rights_invalid_str_mv Derechos Reservados - Universidad Católica de Colombia, 2019
Atribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacional (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
dc.format.mimetype.spa.fl_str_mv application/pdf
dc.publisher.spa.fl_str_mv Universidad Católica de Colombia. Facultad de Psicología
institution Universidad Católica de Colombia
bitstream.url.fl_str_mv https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/54be2569-88e9-4d2c-b441-d2ef59a19567/download
https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/6fd9c46a-7299-44f7-8c52-d21cb19b3336/download
https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/ce2fb8b3-e987-48ca-884e-6e00f54a2185/download
https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/d35223e1-8f54-44e3-b91e-d6874f13bfec/download
https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/9754c994-e9d2-4adf-897a-ffeb6d54529d/download
https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/847c1eb4-42db-4173-9953-d4c7fce8600c/download
bitstream.checksum.fl_str_mv fa61306a4b92a77f67aa56293dbdd501
3ce7be95f40131003ff8d1431b34e960
3da27036ad1c9197591e26e8ee8375f5
465e5418bc93f65fd771f6014056cdb1
0a5ee89b7fdf7c78cba43261d8518c1b
02e22afa18e35968b02ae33617cd0d20
bitstream.checksumAlgorithm.fl_str_mv MD5
MD5
MD5
MD5
MD5
MD5
repository.name.fl_str_mv Repositorio Institucional Universidad Católica de Colombia - RIUCaC
repository.mail.fl_str_mv bdigital@metabiblioteca.com
_version_ 1814256350981521408
spelling Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabrielafd26d894-b23e-45a2-b5f7-7ec57992d7f7-1Reynoso-Alcántara, Vicentab907720d-3f0e-4b94-8bf5-2c4b892b7d23-12019-09-04T14:55:52Z2019-09-04T14:55:52Z2019-12La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el compromiso social (en sus dimensiones de contacto, apoyo y conflicto social) y algunos procesos cognitivos (como flexibilidad mental, planeación, fluidez verbal, velocidad de procesamiento y comprensión verbal) en jóvenes universitarios. Para esto, se realizó un estudio correlacional con un diseño transversal en una muestra de 49 estudiantes. El análisis de correlación de Spearman reveló correlaciones entre contacto social y fluidez verbal (r = -.299, p = .037), apoyo social y planeación (r = .368, p = .009), y conflicto social y velocidad de procesamiento (r = .306, p = .032). Estos resultados perfilan una asociación entre el compromiso social y algunas funciones cognitivas. A pesar de que los resultados no son del todo concluyentes, queda claro que se necesita de un estudio más detallado de estos procesos.Greater social engagement is commonly associated with more efficient cognitive processing, as it improves cognitive reserve. The aim of this research is to assess the relationship between social engagement (in the dimensions of social contact, support and conflict) and certain cognitive skills (cognitive flexibility, planning, verbal fluency, processing speed, and verbal comprehension) in young college students. This cross-sectional rank correlation study was carried out in a sample of 49 college students. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient showed relationship between: social contact and verbal fluency (r = -.299, p = .037); social support and planning (r = .368, p = .009); and social conflict and processing speed (r = .306, p = .032). The results outline a measurable relationship between social engagement and certain cognitive skills, even when said results are not entirely conclusive. Thus, the need for more detailed studies on these processes becomes clear.A literatura científica indica que um maior compromisso social está relacionado com um pensamento cognitivo mais eficiente, devido ao fato de que tal compromisso aumenta a reserva cognitiva. Levando isso em consideração, o objetivo da presente pesquisa foi identificar a relação existente entre o compromisso social (em suas dimensões de contato, apoio e conflito social) e alguns processos cognitivos (como flexibilidade mental, planejamento, fluidez verbal, velocidade de processamento e compreensão verbal) em jovens universitários. Para tanto, realizou-se um estudo correlacional com um desenho transversal em uma amostra de 49 estudantes. A análise de correlação de Spearman revelou correlações entre contato social e fluidez verbal (r = -.299, p = .037), apoio social e planejamento (r = .368, p = .009) e conflito social e velocidade de processamento (r = .306, p = .032). Esses resultados constituem uma associação entre o compromisso social e algumas funções cognitivas. Apesar dos resultados não serem completamente conclusivos, fica clara a necessidade de um estudo mais detalhado desses processos.application/pdfTrejo-Becerra, E., & Reynoso-Alcantara, V. (2019). Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 22(2), 218-229. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.110123-9155https://hdl.handle.net/10983/23661spaUniversidad Católica de Colombia. Facultad de PsicologíaActa Colombiana de Psicología, Vol. 22 no. 2 (jul.-dic. 2019); p. 218-240Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056.Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Wilson, R. S., Bienias, J. L., & Evans, D. A. (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63(12), 2322-2326. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147473.04043.B3.Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social Disengagement and Incident Cognitive Decline in Community-Dwelling Elderly Persons. Annals of Internal Medicine, 131(3), 165-173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002.Bennett, D. A., Arnold, S. E., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C. & Schneider, J. A. (2014). Cognitive and social lifestyle: Links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathological; 127(1), 137-150. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2.Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., & Wilson, R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer's disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406-412. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70417-3.Berkman, L. F. (2000). Which influences cognitive function: Living alone or being alone? The Lancet, 355(9212), 1291-1292. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02107-3.Cornwell, B. & Laumann, E. O. (2015). The health benefits of network growth: New evidence from a national survey of older adults. Social Science Medicine; 125, 94-106. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.011.Cremonte, M., Ledesma, R., Cherpitel, C., & Borges, G. (2010). Psychometric properties of alcohol screening tests in the emergency department in Argentina, Mexico and the United States. Addictive Behaviors, 35(9), 818-25. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.03.021.Flores, J. C., Ostrosky, F., & Lozano, A. (2014). BANFE-2, Batería neurosicológica de funciones ejecutivas y lóbulos frontales [BANFE-2, Neurosychological battery of executive functions and frontal lobes]. México: Manual Moderno.Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology , 3(6), 343-353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7.Gardner, P. (2014). The role of social engagement and identity in community mobility among older adults aging in place. Disability and Rehabilitation, 36(15), 1249-1257. doi:10.3109/09638288.2013.837970.Gariépy, G., Honkaniemi, H., & Quesnel-Vallée, A. (2016). Social support and protection from depression: systematic review of current findings in Western countries. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 284-293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.169094.z.Grossetti, M. (2009). ¿Qué es una relación social? Un conjunto de mediaciones diádicas [What is a social relation? A set of dyadic mediations]. REDES-Revista Hispana Para El Análisis de Redes Sociales, 6(2), 44-62. doi: 10.5565/REV/REDES.364.Hernández, R., Fernández, C., & Baptista, P. (2010). Metodología de la investigación (5a ed.) [Investigation Methodology]. México: Mcgraw-Hill Educación.Holtzman, R. E., Rebok, G. W., Saczynski, J. S., Kouzis, A. C., Wilcox Doyle, K., & Eaton, W. W. (2004). Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59(6), 278-284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P278.Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472-503. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.4.472.Krueger, K., & Wilson, R. (2009). Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1080/03610730802545028.SOCIAL.La Fleur, C. G., & Salthouse, T. A. (2017). Which aspects of social support are associated with which cognitive abilities for which people? The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 72(6), 1006-1016. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv119.Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Bigler, E., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Liao, J. & Scholes, S. (2017). Association of social support and cognitive aging modified by sex and relationship type: A prospective investigation in the English longitudinal study of ageing. American Journal of Epidemiology; 186(7), 787-795. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx142.Liao, J., Head, J., Kumari, M., Stansfeld, S., Kivimaki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., & Brunner, E. J. (2014). Negative aspects of close relationships as risk factors for cognitive aging. American Journal of Epidemi; 180(11), 1118-1125. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu236.Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., & Pereiro, A. X. (2014). El nivel de vocabulario como indicador de reserva cognitiva en la evaluación del deterioro cognitivo ligero [The level of vocabulary as an indicator of cognitive reserve in the evaluation of mild cognitive impairment]. Anales de Psicología, 30, 1115-1121. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.158481.Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K., Rorden, C., Pickrd, J., & Robertson, I. H. (2003). Enhancing the Sensitivity of a Sustained Attention Task to Frontal Damage: Convergent Clinical and Functional Imaging Evidence. Neurocase, 9(4), 340-349. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.4.340.15553.Marioni, R. E., Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Dartigues, J.-F., & Jacqmin-Gadda, H. (2015). Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: a 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health, 15(1), 1089. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2426-6.Mayor, J., Amador, F., & Ramírez, I. (2008). La reserva cognitiva mejora la velocidad de procesamiento de los componentes centrales del tiempo de reacción en adultos mayores pero no en jóvenes [Cognitive reserve improves the processing speed of the central components of reaction time in older adults but not in young people]. Revista Cubana de Salud Y Trabajo, 9(1), 7-18.McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Review, 87(3), 873-904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006.Nelson, L. A., Noonan, C. J., Goldberg, J., & Buchwald, D. S. (2013). Social Engagement and Physical and Cognitive Health Among American Indian Participants in the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 28(4), 453-463. doi: 10.1007/s10823-013-9213-6.Nithianantharajah, J., & Hannan, A. J. (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 89(4), 369-382. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.001.Pillemer, S. C., & Holtzer, R. (2016). The differential relationships of dimensions of perceived social support with cognitive function among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 20(7), 727-735. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1033683.Rodriguez-Laso, A., Zunzunegui, M. V., & Otero, A. (2007). The effect of social relationships on survival in elderly residents of a Southern European community: A cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 7, 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-7-19.Rosselli, M., Jurado, M. B., & Matute, E. (2008). Las Funciones Ejecutivas a través de la Vida [Executive Functions through Life]. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría Y Neurociencias, 8(1), 23-46. Retrieved from http://neurociencias.udea.edu.co/revista/PDF/REVNEURO_vol8_num1_5.pdf.Saczynski, J. S. (2006). The Effect of Social Engagement on Incident Dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemi ology , 163(5), 433-440. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj061.Scarmeas, N., & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section A), 25(5), 625-633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576.Seeman, T., Lusignolo, T., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychology, 20(4), 243-255. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.243.Seeman, T., Miller-Martinez, D., Stein Merkin, S., Lachman, M., Tun, P., & Karlamangla, A. (2011). Histories of Social Engagement and Adult Cognition: Midlife in the U.S. Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B (S1), i141-i152. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq091.Segel-Karpas, D. & Lachman, M. E. (2016). Social contact and cognitive functioning: The role of personality. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences , 73(6), 974-984. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw079.Skakkebœk, A., Moore, P.J., Pedersen, A.D., Bojesen, A., Kristensen, M. K., Fedder, J., Laurberg, P., Hertz, J. M., 0stergaard, J. R., Wallentin, M. & Gravholt, C. H. (2017). The role of genes, intelligence, personality, and social engagement in cognitive performance in Klinefelter syndrome. Brain and Behavior, 7(3), e00645. doi: 10.1002/brb3.645.Soto-Añari, M., Flores-Valdivia, G., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2013). Nivel de lectura como medida de reserva cognitiva en adultos mayores [Reading level as a measure of cognitive reserve in older adults]. Revista de Neurologia, 56(2), 79-85.SPSS Inc. Edición 2007. SPSS para Windows, Version 16.0. Chicago, SPSS Inc.Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004.Tun, P. A., Miller-Martinez, D., Lachman, M. E., & Seeman, T. (2013). Social strain and executive function across the lifespan: The dark (and light) sides of social engagement. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 20(3), 320-338. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.707173.Umberson, D., & Karas, J. (2010). Social Relationships and Health: A Flashpoint for Health Policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(S1), S54-S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501.Whechsler, D. (2012). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV, WAIS-IV [Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV, WAIS-IV]. Manual de aplicación. Mexico, D.F.: Manual Moderno.Wilson, R. S., Boyle P. A., James, B. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Negative social interactions and risk of mild cognitive impairment in old age. Neuropsychology; 29(4), 561-570. doi: 10.1037/neu0000154.Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., Tang, Y, & Bennett, D. A. (2007). Loneliness and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(2), 234-240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234.Zainab, N. & Naz, H. (2017). Daily living functioning, social engagement and wellness of older adults. Psychology, Community & Health, 6(1), 93-102. doi: 10.5964/pch.v6i1.213.Zhou, Z., Wang, P., & Fang, Y. (2018). Social Engagement and Its Change are Associated with Dementia Risk among Chinese Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1551. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17879-w.Zuelsdorff, M. L., Koscik, R. L., Okonkwo, O. C., Peppard, P. E., Hermann, B. P., Sager, M. A., Johnson, S. C. & Engelman, C. D. (2018). Reliability of a novel social activity questionnaire: Perceived social support and verbal interaction in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention. Journal of Aging and Health; 30(2), 305-320. doi: 10.1177/0898264316674812.Zunzunegui, M.-V., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences , 58(2), S93-S100. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S93.Derechos Reservados - Universidad Católica de Colombia, 2019info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessAtribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacional (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0)https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2RESERVA COGNITIVARELACIONES INTERPERSONALESCOGNICIÓNCOGNITIVE RESERVEINTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPSCOGNITIONRESERVA COGNITIVARELAÇÕES INTERPESSOAISCOGNIÇÃOCompromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitariosSocial engagement as a measurement of cognitive reserve and how it relates to different cognitive skills in college studentsCompromisso social como medida de reserva cognitiva e sua relação com diferentes processos cognitivos em jovens universitáriosArtículo de revistahttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1Textinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlehttp://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ARTinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionhttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85PublicationORIGINAL2036-17174-1-PB.pdf2036-17174-1-PB.pdfArticulo principalapplication/pdf860730https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/54be2569-88e9-4d2c-b441-d2ef59a19567/downloadfa61306a4b92a77f67aa56293dbdd501MD512036-17175-1-PB.pdf2036-17175-1-PB.pdfArticulo principalapplication/pdf855280https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/6fd9c46a-7299-44f7-8c52-d21cb19b3336/download3ce7be95f40131003ff8d1431b34e960MD52TEXT2036-17174-1-PB.pdf.txt2036-17174-1-PB.pdf.txtExtracted texttext/plain56460https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/ce2fb8b3-e987-48ca-884e-6e00f54a2185/download3da27036ad1c9197591e26e8ee8375f5MD532036-17175-1-PB.pdf.txt2036-17175-1-PB.pdf.txtExtracted texttext/plain52999https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/d35223e1-8f54-44e3-b91e-d6874f13bfec/download465e5418bc93f65fd771f6014056cdb1MD55THUMBNAIL2036-17174-1-PB.pdf.jpg2036-17174-1-PB.pdf.jpgRIUCACimage/jpeg25034https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/9754c994-e9d2-4adf-897a-ffeb6d54529d/download0a5ee89b7fdf7c78cba43261d8518c1bMD542036-17175-1-PB.pdf.jpg2036-17175-1-PB.pdf.jpgRIUCACimage/jpeg25027https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/847c1eb4-42db-4173-9953-d4c7fce8600c/download02e22afa18e35968b02ae33617cd0d20MD5610983/23661oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/236612023-03-24 17:05:38.041https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/Derechos Reservados - Universidad Católica de Colombia, 2019https://repository.ucatolica.edu.coRepositorio Institucional Universidad Católica de Colombia - RIUCaCbdigital@metabiblioteca.com