Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.

La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el co...

Full description

Autores:
Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcantara, Vicenta
Tipo de recurso:
Article of investigation
Fecha de publicación:
2019
Institución:
Universidad Católica de Colombia
Repositorio:
RIUCaC - Repositorio U. Católica
Idioma:
spa
OAI Identifier:
oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/28448
Acceso en línea:
https://hdl.handle.net/10983/28448
https://doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
Palabra clave:
Cognitive reserve
Interpersonal relationships
Cognition
Reserva cognitiva
Relaciones interpersonales
Cognición
Reserva cognitiva
Relações interpessoais
Cognição
Rights
openAccess
License
Eva Gabriela Trejo Becerra - 2019
id UCATOLICA2_995a579330047d9c49a5a9d66b8987dc
oai_identifier_str oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/28448
network_acronym_str UCATOLICA2
network_name_str RIUCaC - Repositorio U. Católica
repository_id_str
dc.title.spa.fl_str_mv Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
dc.title.translated.eng.fl_str_mv Social engagement as a measurement of cognitive reserve and how it relates to different cognitive skills in college students.
title Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
spellingShingle Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
Cognitive reserve
Interpersonal relationships
Cognition
Reserva cognitiva
Relaciones interpersonales
Cognición
Reserva cognitiva
Relações interpessoais
Cognição
title_short Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
title_full Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
title_fullStr Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
title_full_unstemmed Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
title_sort Compromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.
dc.creator.fl_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcantara, Vicenta
dc.contributor.author.spa.fl_str_mv Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabriela
Reynoso-Alcantara, Vicenta
dc.subject.eng.fl_str_mv Cognitive reserve
Interpersonal relationships
Cognition
topic Cognitive reserve
Interpersonal relationships
Cognition
Reserva cognitiva
Relaciones interpersonales
Cognición
Reserva cognitiva
Relações interpessoais
Cognição
dc.subject.spa.fl_str_mv Reserva cognitiva
Relaciones interpersonales
Cognición
Reserva cognitiva
Relações interpessoais
Cognição
description La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el compromiso social (en sus dimensiones de contacto, apoyo y conflicto social) y algunos procesos cognitivos (como flexibilidad mental, planeación, fluidez verbal, velocidad de procesamiento y comprensión verbal) en jóvenes universitarios. Para esto, se realizó un estudio correlacional con un diseño transversal en una muestra de 49 estudiantes. El análisis de correlación de Spearman reveló correlaciones entre contacto social y fluidez verbal (r = .299, p = .037), apoyo social y planeación (r = .368, p = .009), y conflicto social y velocidad de procesamiento (r = .306, p = .032). Estos resultados perfilan una asociación entre el compromiso social y algunas funciones cognitivas. A pesar de que los resultados no son del todo concluyentes, queda claro que se necesita de un estudio más detallado de estos procesos.
publishDate 2019
dc.date.accessioned.none.fl_str_mv 2019-07-07 23:04:34
2023-01-23T15:43:04Z
dc.date.available.none.fl_str_mv 2019-07-07 23:04:34
2023-01-23T15:43:04Z
dc.date.issued.none.fl_str_mv 2019-07-07
dc.type.spa.fl_str_mv Artículo de revista
dc.type.coar.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.type.coarversion.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85
dc.type.content.spa.fl_str_mv Text
dc.type.driver.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.local.eng.fl_str_mv Journal article
dc.type.redcol.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ART
dc.type.version.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion
format http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
status_str publishedVersion
dc.identifier.doi.none.fl_str_mv 10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
dc.identifier.eissn.none.fl_str_mv 1909-9711
dc.identifier.issn.none.fl_str_mv 0123-9155
dc.identifier.uri.none.fl_str_mv https://hdl.handle.net/10983/28448
dc.identifier.url.none.fl_str_mv https://doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
identifier_str_mv 10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
1909-9711
0123-9155
url https://hdl.handle.net/10983/28448
https://doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11
dc.language.iso.spa.fl_str_mv spa
language spa
dc.relation.bitstream.none.fl_str_mv https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2583
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2700
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2584
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2707
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2684
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2872
https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2877
dc.relation.citationedition.spa.fl_str_mv Núm. 2 , Año 2019 : ACTA COLOMBIANA DE PSICOLOGÏA
dc.relation.citationendpage.none.fl_str_mv 240
dc.relation.citationissue.spa.fl_str_mv 2
dc.relation.citationstartpage.none.fl_str_mv 218
dc.relation.citationvolume.spa.fl_str_mv 22
dc.relation.ispartofjournal.spa.fl_str_mv Acta Colombiana de Psicología
dc.relation.references.spa.fl_str_mv Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056
Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Wilson, R. S., Bienias, J. L., & Evans, D. A. (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63(12), 2322-2326. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147473.04043.B3
Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social Disengagement and Incident Cognitive Decline in Community-Dwelling Elderly Persons. Annals of Internal Medicine, 131(3), 165-173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002
Bennett, D. A., Arnold, S. E., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C., & Schneider, J. A. (2014). Cognitive and social lifestyle: Links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathological, 127(1), 137-150. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., & Wilson, R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer?s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406-412. doi: 10.1016/S147/44422(06)70417-3
Berkman, L. F. (2000). Which influences cognitive function: Living alone or being alone? The Lancet, 355(9212), 1291-1292. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02107-3
Cornwell, B., & Laumann, E. O. (2015). The health benefits of network growth: New evidence from a national survey of older adults. Social Science Medicine, 125, 94-106. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.011
Cremonte, M., Ledesma, R., Cherpitel, C., & Borges, G. (2010). Psychometric properties of alcohol screening tests in the emergency department in Argentina, Mexico and the United States. Addictive Behaviors, 35(9), 818-25. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.03.021
Flores, J. C., Ostrosky, F., & Lozano, A. (2014). BANFE-2, Batería neurosicológica de funciones ejecutivas y lóbulos frontales. México: Manual Moderno.
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3(6), 343-353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-422(04)00767-7
Gardner, P. (2014). The role of social engagement and identity in community mobility among older adults aging in place. Disability and Rehabilitation, 36(15), 1249-1257. doi:10.3109/09638288.2013.837970
Gariépy, G., Honkaniemi, H., & Quesnel-Vallée, A. (2016). Social support and protection from depression: systematic review of current findings in Western countries. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 284-293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.169094
Grossetti, M. (2009). ¿Qué es una relación social? Un conjunto de mediaciones diádicas. REDES-Revista Hispana Para El Análisis de Redes Sociales, 6(2), 44-62. doi: 10.5565/REV/REDES.364
Hernández, R., Fernández, C., & Baptista, P. (2010). Metodología de la investigación (5.a ed.). México: Mcgraw-Hill Educación.
Holtzman, R. E., Rebok, G. W., Saczynski, J. S., Kouzis, A. C., Wilcox Doyle, K., & Eaton, W. W. (2004). Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59(6), 278-284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P278
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472-503. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.4.472
Krueger, K., & Wilson, R. (2009). Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1080/03610730802545028
La Fleur, C. G., & Salthouse, T. A. (2017). Which aspects of social support are associated with which cognitive abilities for which people? The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 72(6), 1006-1016. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv119
Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Bigler, E., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment (5.a ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Liao, J., & Scholes, S. (2017). Association of social support and cognitive aging modified by sex and relationship type: A prospective investigation in the English longitudinal study of ageing. American Journal of Epidemiology, 186(7), 787-795. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx142
Liao, J., Head, J., Kumari, M., Stansfeld, S., Kivimaki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., & Brunner, E. J. (2014). Negative aspects of close relationships as risk factors for cognitive aging. American Journal of Epidemi, 180(11), 1118-1125. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu236
Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., & Pereiro, A. X. (2014). El nivel de vocabulario como indicador de reserva cognitiva en la evaluación del deterioro cognitivo ligero. Anales de Psicología, 30(3), 1115-1121. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.158481
Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K., ? Robertson, I. H. (2003). Enhancing the Sensitivity of a Sustained Attention Task to Frontal Damage: Convergent Clinical and Functional Imaging Evidence. Neurocase, 9(4), 340-349. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.4.340.15553
Marioni, R. E., Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Dartigues, J. F., & Jacqmin-Gadda, H. (2015). Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: a 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health, 15(1), 1089. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2426-6
Mayor, J., Amador, F., & Ramírez, I. (2008). La reserva cognitiva mejora la velocidad de procesamiento de los componentes centrales del tiempo de reacción en adultos mayores pero no en jóvenes. Revista Cubana de Salud y Trabajo, 9(1), 7-18. Recuperado de http://bvs.sld.cu/revistas/rst/
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Review, 87(3), 873-904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006
Nelson, L. A., Noonan, C. J., Goldberg, J., & Buchwald, D. S. (2013). Social Engagement and Physical and Cognitive Health Among American Indian Participants in the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 28(4), 453-463. doi: 10.1007/s10823-013-9213-6
Nithianantharajah, J., & Hannan, A. J. (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 89(4), 369-382. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.001
Pillemer, S. C., & Holtzer, R. (2016). The differential relationships of dimensions of perceived social support with cognitive function among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 20(7), 727-735. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1033683
Rodriguez-Laso, A., Zunzunegui, M. V., & Otero, A. (2007). The effect of social relationships on survival in elderly residents of a Southern European community: A cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 7(19), 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-7-19
Rosselli, M., Jurado, M. B., & Matute, E. (2008). Las Funciones Ejecutivas a través de la Vida. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría Y Neurociencias, 8(1), 23-46. Recuperado de http://neurociencias.udea.edu.co/revista/PDF/REVNEURO_vol8_num1_5.pdf
Saczynski, J. S. (2006). The Effect of Social Engagement on Incident Dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 163(5), 433-440. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj061
Scarmeas, N., & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section A), 25(5), 625-633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576
Seeman, T., Lusignolo, T., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychology, 20(4), 243-255. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.243
Seeman, T., Miller-Martinez, D., Stein Merkin, S., Lachman, M., Tun, P., & Karlamangla, A. (2011). Histories of Social Engagement and Adult Cognition: Midlife in the U.S. Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B (S1), i141-i152. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq091
Segel-Karpas, D., & Lachman, M. E. (2016). Social contact and cognitive functioning: The role of personality. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 73(6), 974-984. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw079
Skakkebæk, A., Moore, P. J., Pedersen, A. D., Bojesen, A., Kristensen, M. K., Fedder, J., ? Gravholt, C. H. (2017). The role of genes, intelligence, personality, and social engagement in cognitive performance in Klinefelter syndrome. Brain and Behavior, 7(3), e00645. doi: 10.1002/brb3.645
Soto-Añari, M., Flores-Valdivia, G., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2013). Nivel de lectura como medida de reserva cognitive en adultos mayores. Revista de Neurologia, 56(2), 79-85.
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004
Tun, P. A., Miller-Martinez, D., Lachman, M. E., & Seeman, T. (2013). Social strain and executive function across the lifespan: The dark (and light) sides of social engagement. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 20(3), 320-338. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.707173
Umberson, D., & Karas, J. (2010). Social Relationships and Health: A Flashpoint for Health Policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(S1), S54-S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501
Wechsler, D. (2012). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV, WAIS-IV. Manual de aplicacio´n. Mexico, D. F.: Manual Moderno.
Wilson, R. S., Boyle P. A., James, B. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Negative social interactions and risk of mild cognitive impairment in old age. Neuropsychology, 29(4), 561-570. doi: 10.1037/neu0000154
Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., ? Bennett, D. A. (2007).Loneliness and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(2), 234-240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234
Zainab, N., & Naz, H. (2017). Daily living functioning, social engagement and wellness of older adults. Psychology, Community & Health, 6(1), 93-102. doi: 10.5964/pch.v6i1.213
Zhou, Z., Wang, P., & Fang, Y. (2018). Social Engagement and Its Change are Associated with Dementia Risk among Chinese Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1551. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17879-w
Zuelsdorff, M. L., Koscik, R. L., Okonkwo, O. C., Peppard, P. E., Hermann, B. P., Sager, M. A., ? Engelman, C. D. (2018). Reliability of a novel social activity questionnaire: Perceived social support and verbal interaction in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention. Journal of Aging and Health, 30(2), 305-320. doi: 10.1177/0898264316674812
Zunzunegui, M. V., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(2), S93-S100. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S93
dc.rights.spa.fl_str_mv Eva Gabriela Trejo Becerra - 2019
dc.rights.accessrights.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rights.coar.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
dc.rights.uri.spa.fl_str_mv https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
rights_invalid_str_mv Eva Gabriela Trejo Becerra - 2019
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
dc.format.mimetype.spa.fl_str_mv application/pdf
text/html
application/pdf
text/html
application/xml
application/pdf
application/vnd.openxmlformats-officedocument.wordprocessingml.document
dc.publisher.spa.fl_str_mv Universidad Católica de Colombia
dc.source.spa.fl_str_mv https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/2036
institution Universidad Católica de Colombia
bitstream.url.fl_str_mv https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/498d5c5b-90ac-405a-abeb-732d78f394cb/download
bitstream.checksum.fl_str_mv f99fcb9735375f72de19969888648cf3
bitstream.checksumAlgorithm.fl_str_mv MD5
repository.name.fl_str_mv Repositorio Institucional Universidad Católica de Colombia - RIUCaC
repository.mail.fl_str_mv bdigital@metabiblioteca.com
_version_ 1812183301032509440
spelling Trejo-Becerra, Eva Gabrielafd26d894-b23e-45a2-b5f7-7ec57992d7f7Reynoso-Alcantara, Vicenta39bca27f-35c2-489a-99e7-79c05cac570a3002019-07-07 23:04:342023-01-23T15:43:04Z2019-07-07 23:04:342023-01-23T15:43:04Z2019-07-07La literatura científica indica que un mayor compromiso social se relaciona con un procesamiento cognitivo más eficiente, debido a que dicho compromiso incrementa la reserva cognitiva. Teniendo esto en cuenta, el objetivo de la presente investigación fue identificar la relación existente entre el compromiso social (en sus dimensiones de contacto, apoyo y conflicto social) y algunos procesos cognitivos (como flexibilidad mental, planeación, fluidez verbal, velocidad de procesamiento y comprensión verbal) en jóvenes universitarios. Para esto, se realizó un estudio correlacional con un diseño transversal en una muestra de 49 estudiantes. El análisis de correlación de Spearman reveló correlaciones entre contacto social y fluidez verbal (r = .299, p = .037), apoyo social y planeación (r = .368, p = .009), y conflicto social y velocidad de procesamiento (r = .306, p = .032). Estos resultados perfilan una asociación entre el compromiso social y algunas funciones cognitivas. A pesar de que los resultados no son del todo concluyentes, queda claro que se necesita de un estudio más detallado de estos procesos.Greater social engagement is commonly associated with more efficient cognitive processing, as it improves cognitive reserve. The aim of this research is to assess the relationship between social engagement (in the dimensions of social contact, support and conflict) and certain cognitive skills (cognitive flexibility, planning, verbal fluency, processing speed, and verbal comprehension) in young college students. This cross-sectional rank correlation study was carried out in a sample of 49 college students. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient showed relationship between: social contact and verbal fluency (r = .299, p = .037); social support and planning (r = .368, p = .009); and social conflict and processing speed (r = .306, p = .032). The results outline a measurable relationship between social engagement and certain cognitive skills, even when said results are not entirely conclusive. Thus, the need for more detailed studies on these processes becomes clear.application/pdftext/htmlapplication/pdftext/htmlapplication/xmlapplication/pdfapplication/vnd.openxmlformats-officedocument.wordprocessingml.document10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.111909-97110123-9155https://hdl.handle.net/10983/28448https://doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2019.22.2.11spaUniversidad Católica de Colombiahttps://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2583https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2700https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2584https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2707https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2684https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2872https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/download/2036/2877Núm. 2 , Año 2019 : ACTA COLOMBIANA DE PSICOLOGÏA240221822Acta Colombiana de PsicologíaAdolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056Barnes, L. L., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Wilson, R. S., Bienias, J. L., & Evans, D. A. (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63(12), 2322-2326. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000147473.04043.B3Bassuk, S. S., Glass, T. A., & Berkman, L. F. (1999). Social Disengagement and Incident Cognitive Decline in Community-Dwelling Elderly Persons. Annals of Internal Medicine, 131(3), 165-173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002Bennett, D. A., Arnold, S. E., Valenzuela, M. J., Brayne, C., & Schneider, J. A. (2014). Cognitive and social lifestyle: Links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathological, 127(1), 137-150. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., & Wilson, R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer?s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406-412. doi: 10.1016/S147/44422(06)70417-3Berkman, L. F. (2000). Which influences cognitive function: Living alone or being alone? The Lancet, 355(9212), 1291-1292. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02107-3Cornwell, B., & Laumann, E. O. (2015). The health benefits of network growth: New evidence from a national survey of older adults. Social Science Medicine, 125, 94-106. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.011Cremonte, M., Ledesma, R., Cherpitel, C., & Borges, G. (2010). Psychometric properties of alcohol screening tests in the emergency department in Argentina, Mexico and the United States. Addictive Behaviors, 35(9), 818-25. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.03.021Flores, J. C., Ostrosky, F., & Lozano, A. (2014). BANFE-2, Batería neurosicológica de funciones ejecutivas y lóbulos frontales. México: Manual Moderno.Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3(6), 343-353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-422(04)00767-7Gardner, P. (2014). The role of social engagement and identity in community mobility among older adults aging in place. Disability and Rehabilitation, 36(15), 1249-1257. doi:10.3109/09638288.2013.837970Gariépy, G., Honkaniemi, H., & Quesnel-Vallée, A. (2016). Social support and protection from depression: systematic review of current findings in Western countries. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 284-293. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.169094Grossetti, M. (2009). ¿Qué es una relación social? Un conjunto de mediaciones diádicas. REDES-Revista Hispana Para El Análisis de Redes Sociales, 6(2), 44-62. doi: 10.5565/REV/REDES.364Hernández, R., Fernández, C., & Baptista, P. (2010). Metodología de la investigación (5.a ed.). México: Mcgraw-Hill Educación.Holtzman, R. E., Rebok, G. W., Saczynski, J. S., Kouzis, A. C., Wilcox Doyle, K., & Eaton, W. W. (2004). Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59(6), 278-284. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P278Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472-503. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.4.472Krueger, K., & Wilson, R. (2009). Social engagement and cognitive function in old age. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 1-12. doi: 10.1080/03610730802545028La Fleur, C. G., & Salthouse, T. A. (2017). Which aspects of social support are associated with which cognitive abilities for which people? The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 72(6), 1006-1016. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv119Lezak, M., Howieson, D., Bigler, E., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological Assessment (5.a ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Liao, J., & Scholes, S. (2017). Association of social support and cognitive aging modified by sex and relationship type: A prospective investigation in the English longitudinal study of ageing. American Journal of Epidemiology, 186(7), 787-795. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx142Liao, J., Head, J., Kumari, M., Stansfeld, S., Kivimaki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., & Brunner, E. J. (2014). Negative aspects of close relationships as risk factors for cognitive aging. American Journal of Epidemi, 180(11), 1118-1125. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu236Lojo-Seoane, C., Facal, D., Juncos-Rabadán, O., & Pereiro, A. X. (2014). El nivel de vocabulario como indicador de reserva cognitiva en la evaluación del deterioro cognitivo ligero. Anales de Psicología, 30(3), 1115-1121. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.3.158481Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K., ? Robertson, I. H. (2003). Enhancing the Sensitivity of a Sustained Attention Task to Frontal Damage: Convergent Clinical and Functional Imaging Evidence. Neurocase, 9(4), 340-349. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.4.340.15553Marioni, R. E., Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. E., Dartigues, J. F., & Jacqmin-Gadda, H. (2015). Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: a 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health, 15(1), 1089. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2426-6Mayor, J., Amador, F., & Ramírez, I. (2008). La reserva cognitiva mejora la velocidad de procesamiento de los componentes centrales del tiempo de reacción en adultos mayores pero no en jóvenes. Revista Cubana de Salud y Trabajo, 9(1), 7-18. Recuperado de http://bvs.sld.cu/revistas/rst/McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Review, 87(3), 873-904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006Nelson, L. A., Noonan, C. J., Goldberg, J., & Buchwald, D. S. (2013). Social Engagement and Physical and Cognitive Health Among American Indian Participants in the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 28(4), 453-463. doi: 10.1007/s10823-013-9213-6Nithianantharajah, J., & Hannan, A. J. (2009). The neurobiology of brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity as modulators of brain disorders. Progress in Neurobiology, 89(4), 369-382. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.001Pillemer, S. C., & Holtzer, R. (2016). The differential relationships of dimensions of perceived social support with cognitive function among older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 20(7), 727-735. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1033683Rodriguez-Laso, A., Zunzunegui, M. V., & Otero, A. (2007). The effect of social relationships on survival in elderly residents of a Southern European community: A cohort study. BMC Geriatrics, 7(19), 1-12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-7-19Rosselli, M., Jurado, M. B., & Matute, E. (2008). Las Funciones Ejecutivas a través de la Vida. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría Y Neurociencias, 8(1), 23-46. Recuperado de http://neurociencias.udea.edu.co/revista/PDF/REVNEURO_vol8_num1_5.pdfSaczynski, J. S. (2006). The Effect of Social Engagement on Incident Dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 163(5), 433-440. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj061Scarmeas, N., & Stern, Y. (2003). Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section A), 25(5), 625-633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576Seeman, T., Lusignolo, T., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychology, 20(4), 243-255. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.243Seeman, T., Miller-Martinez, D., Stein Merkin, S., Lachman, M., Tun, P., & Karlamangla, A. (2011). Histories of Social Engagement and Adult Cognition: Midlife in the U.S. Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66B (S1), i141-i152. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq091Segel-Karpas, D., & Lachman, M. E. (2016). Social contact and cognitive functioning: The role of personality. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 73(6), 974-984. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw079Skakkebæk, A., Moore, P. J., Pedersen, A. D., Bojesen, A., Kristensen, M. K., Fedder, J., ? Gravholt, C. H. (2017). The role of genes, intelligence, personality, and social engagement in cognitive performance in Klinefelter syndrome. Brain and Behavior, 7(3), e00645. doi: 10.1002/brb3.645Soto-Añari, M., Flores-Valdivia, G., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2013). Nivel de lectura como medida de reserva cognitive en adultos mayores. Revista de Neurologia, 56(2), 79-85.Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015-2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004Tun, P. A., Miller-Martinez, D., Lachman, M. E., & Seeman, T. (2013). Social strain and executive function across the lifespan: The dark (and light) sides of social engagement. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 20(3), 320-338. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.707173Umberson, D., & Karas, J. (2010). Social Relationships and Health: A Flashpoint for Health Policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(S1), S54-S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501Wechsler, D. (2012). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV, WAIS-IV. Manual de aplicacio´n. Mexico, D. F.: Manual Moderno.Wilson, R. S., Boyle P. A., James, B. D., Leurgans, S. E., Buchman, A. S., & Bennett, D. A. (2015). Negative social interactions and risk of mild cognitive impairment in old age. Neuropsychology, 29(4), 561-570. doi: 10.1037/neu0000154Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., ? Bennett, D. A. (2007).Loneliness and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(2), 234-240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234Zainab, N., & Naz, H. (2017). Daily living functioning, social engagement and wellness of older adults. Psychology, Community & Health, 6(1), 93-102. doi: 10.5964/pch.v6i1.213Zhou, Z., Wang, P., & Fang, Y. (2018). Social Engagement and Its Change are Associated with Dementia Risk among Chinese Older Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1551. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17879-wZuelsdorff, M. L., Koscik, R. L., Okonkwo, O. C., Peppard, P. E., Hermann, B. P., Sager, M. A., ? Engelman, C. D. (2018). Reliability of a novel social activity questionnaire: Perceived social support and verbal interaction in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention. Journal of Aging and Health, 30(2), 305-320. doi: 10.1177/0898264316674812Zunzunegui, M. V., Alvarado, B. E., Del Ser, T., & Otero, A. (2003). Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(2), S93-S100. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S93Eva Gabriela Trejo Becerra - 2019info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/2036Cognitive reserveInterpersonal relationshipsCognitionReserva cognitivaRelaciones interpersonalesCogniciónReserva cognitivaRelações interpessoaisCogniçãoCompromiso social como medida de reserva cognitiva y su relación con distintos procesos cognitivos en jóvenes universitarios.Social engagement as a measurement of cognitive reserve and how it relates to different cognitive skills in college students.Artículo de revistahttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85Textinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articleJournal articlehttp://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ARTinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionPublicationOREORE.xmltext/xml3893https://repository.ucatolica.edu.co/bitstreams/498d5c5b-90ac-405a-abeb-732d78f394cb/downloadf99fcb9735375f72de19969888648cf3MD5110983/28448oai:repository.ucatolica.edu.co:10983/284482023-08-11 12:55:04.847https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/Eva Gabriela Trejo Becerra - 2019https://repository.ucatolica.edu.coRepositorio Institucional Universidad Católica de Colombia - RIUCaCbdigital@metabiblioteca.com