Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders

Background: Adolescent offenders (AOs) are characterized by social-norm transgression and aggressive behaviors. Those traits have been associated with alterations in socio-cognitive processes, including facial emotion recognition. While this would suggest that AOs tend to interpret negative emotiona...

Full description

Autores:
Santamaría-García, Hernando
Ibáñez, Agustín
Montaño, Synella
García, Adolfo Martín
Patiño-Saenz, Michel
Pino, Mariana
Idarraga, Claudia
Baez, Sandra Jimena
Tipo de recurso:
Article of journal
Fecha de publicación:
2019
Institución:
Corporación Universidad de la Costa
Repositorio:
REDICUC - Repositorio CUC
Idioma:
eng
OAI Identifier:
oai:repositorio.cuc.edu.co:11323/3326
Acceso en línea:
https://hdl.handle.net/11323/3326
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/
Palabra clave:
Adolescent offenders
Emotion recognition
Emotion integration
Brain morphology
Disruptive behaviors
Delincuentes adolescentes
Reconocimiento de emociones
Integración de emociones
Morfología cerebral
Conductas disruptivas
Rights
openAccess
License
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
id RCUC2_2f27c6051083c6f50fd4cbcc8342dcec
oai_identifier_str oai:repositorio.cuc.edu.co:11323/3326
network_acronym_str RCUC2
network_name_str REDICUC - Repositorio CUC
repository_id_str
dc.title.spa.fl_str_mv Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
dc.title.translated.spa.fl_str_mv Fuera de contexto, más allá de la cara: vías neuroanatómicas de la integración emocional del lenguaje cara-cuerpo en adolescentes infractores
title Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
spellingShingle Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
Adolescent offenders
Emotion recognition
Emotion integration
Brain morphology
Disruptive behaviors
Delincuentes adolescentes
Reconocimiento de emociones
Integración de emociones
Morfología cerebral
Conductas disruptivas
title_short Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
title_full Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
title_fullStr Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
title_full_unstemmed Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
title_sort Out of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offenders
dc.creator.fl_str_mv Santamaría-García, Hernando
Ibáñez, Agustín
Montaño, Synella
García, Adolfo Martín
Patiño-Saenz, Michel
Pino, Mariana
Idarraga, Claudia
Baez, Sandra Jimena
dc.contributor.author.spa.fl_str_mv Santamaría-García, Hernando
Ibáñez, Agustín
Montaño, Synella
García, Adolfo Martín
Patiño-Saenz, Michel
Pino, Mariana
Idarraga, Claudia
Baez, Sandra Jimena
dc.subject.spa.fl_str_mv Adolescent offenders
Emotion recognition
Emotion integration
Brain morphology
Disruptive behaviors
Delincuentes adolescentes
Reconocimiento de emociones
Integración de emociones
Morfología cerebral
Conductas disruptivas
topic Adolescent offenders
Emotion recognition
Emotion integration
Brain morphology
Disruptive behaviors
Delincuentes adolescentes
Reconocimiento de emociones
Integración de emociones
Morfología cerebral
Conductas disruptivas
description Background: Adolescent offenders (AOs) are characterized by social-norm transgression and aggressive behaviors. Those traits have been associated with alterations in socio-cognitive processes, including facial emotion recognition. While this would suggest that AOs tend to interpret negative emotional cues as threatening information, most research has relied on context-free stimuli, thus failing to directly track integrative processes typical of everyday cognition. Methods: In this study, we assessed the impact of body language and surrounding context on facial emotion recognition in AOs and non-offenders (NOs). We recruited 35 AOs from a reform school for young male offenders and 30 NOs matched for age and sex with the former group. All participants completed a well-validated task aimed to determine how contextual cues (i.e., emotional body language and surrounding context) influence facial emotion recognition through the use of congruent and incongruent combinations of facial and bodily emotional information. Results: This study showed that AOs tend to overvalue bodily and contextual signals in emotion recognition, with poorer facial-emotion categorization and increased sensitivity to context information in incongruent face-body scenarios. This pattern was associated with executive dysfunctions and disruptive behaviors, as well as with gray matter (GM) of brain regions supporting body-face recognition [fusiform gyrus (FG)], emotion processing [cingulate cortex (CC), superior temporal gyrus (STG)], contextual integration (precuneus, STG), and motor resonance [cerebellum, supplementary motor area (SMA)]. Discussion: Together, our results pave the way for a better understanding of the neurocognitive association between contextual emotion recognition, behavioral regulation, cognitive control, and externalized behaviors in AOs.
publishDate 2019
dc.date.accessioned.none.fl_str_mv 2019-05-15T12:51:27Z
dc.date.available.none.fl_str_mv 2019-05-15T12:51:27Z
dc.date.issued.none.fl_str_mv 2019-02-07
dc.type.spa.fl_str_mv Artículo de revista
dc.type.coar.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.type.coar.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
dc.type.content.spa.fl_str_mv Text
dc.type.driver.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/article
dc.type.redcol.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ART
dc.type.version.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersion
format http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501
status_str acceptedVersion
dc.identifier.issn.spa.fl_str_mv 1662-5153
dc.identifier.uri.spa.fl_str_mv https://hdl.handle.net/11323/3326
dc.identifier.instname.spa.fl_str_mv Corporación Universidad de la Costa
dc.identifier.reponame.spa.fl_str_mv REDICUC - Repositorio CUC
dc.identifier.repourl.spa.fl_str_mv https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/
identifier_str_mv 1662-5153
Corporación Universidad de la Costa
REDICUC - Repositorio CUC
url https://hdl.handle.net/11323/3326
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/
dc.language.iso.none.fl_str_mv eng
language eng
dc.relation.ispartof.spa.fl_str_mv DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00034
dc.relation.references.spa.fl_str_mv Adolfi, F., Couto, B., Richter, F., Decety, J., Lopez, J., Sigman, M., et al. (2016). Convergence of interoception, emotion, and social cognition: a twofold fMRI meta-analysis and lesion approach. Cortex 88, 124–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex. 2016.12.019
Ahmed, S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt, A., and Sebastian, C. L. (2015). Neurocognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006
Amoruso, L., Gelormini, C., Aboitiz, F., Alvarez Gonzalez, M., Manes, F., Cardona, J. F., et al. (2013). N400 ERPs for actions: building meaning in context. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:57. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00057
Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage 11, 805–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582
Aviezer, H., Bentin, S., Hassin, R. R., Meschino, W. S., Kennedy, J., Grewal, S., et al. (2009). Not on the face alone: perception of contextualized face expressions in Huntington’s disease. Brain 132, 1633–1644. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp067
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., et al. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or Afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychol. Sci. 19, 724–732. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02148.x
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., and Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science 338, 1225–1229. doi: 10.1126/science.1224313
Bachmann, J., Munzert, J., and Krüger, B. (2018). Neural underpinnings of the perception of emotional states derived from biological human motion: a review of neuroimaging research. Front. Psychol. 9:1763. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01763
Baez, S., García, A. M., and Ibanez, A. (2017a). The social context network model in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 30, 379–396. doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_443
Baez, S., Herrera, E., García, A., Manes, F., Young, L., and Ibáñez, A. (2017b). Outcome-oriented moral evaluation in terrorists. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0165. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0165
Baez, S., Herrera, E., García, A. M., Huepe, D., Santamaría-García, H., and Ibáñez, A. (2018a). Increased moral condemnation of accidental harm in institutionalized adolescents. Sci. Rep. 8:11609. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29956-9
Baez, S., Pino, M., Berrío, M., Santamaría-García, H., Sedeño, L., García, A. M., et al. (2018b). Corticostriatal signatures of schadenfreude: evidence from Huntington’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 89, 112–116. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-316055
Baez, S., and Ibanez, A. (2014). The effects of context processing on social cognition impairments in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Front. Neurosci. 8:270. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00270
Baez, S., Ibanez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., Perez, A., Roca, M., Manes, F., et al. (2014). The utility of IFS (INECO Frontal Screening) for the detection of executive dysfunction in adults with bipolar disorder and ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 216, 269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.020
Baez, S., Rattazzi, A., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Torralva, T., Vigliecca, N. S., Decety, J., et al. (2012). Integrating intention and context: assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:302. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00302
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., and Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 286–290. doi: 10.1177/09637214114 22522
Best, M., Williams, J. M., and Coccaro, E. F. (2002). Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 8448–8453. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1126 04099
Blair, R. J., Colledge, E., Murray, L., and Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 491–498. doi: 10.1023/A:1012225108281
Bowen, K. L., Morgan, J. E., Moore, S. C., and van Goozen, S. H. M. (2014). Young offenders’ emotion recognition dysfunction across emotion intensities: explaining variation using psychopathic traits, conduct disorder and offense severity. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 36, 60–73. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013- 9368-z
Brazil, I. A., Maes, J. H., Scheper, I., Bulten, B. H., Kessels, R. P., Verkes, R. J., et al. (2013). Reversal deficits in individuals with psychopathy in explici but not implicit learning conditions. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 38, E13–E20. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120152
Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., and Schweitzer, J. (2009). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics 123, 1472–1476. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1406
Budhiraja, M., Savic, I., Lindner, P., Jokinen, J., Tiihonen, J., and Hodgins, S. (2017). Brain structure abnormalities in young women who presented conduct disorder in childhood/adolescence. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 869–885. doi: 10.3758/s13415-017-0519-7
Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Cohen Kadosh, K., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1654–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.011
Burra, N., Hervais-Adelman, A., Celeghin, A., de Gelder, B., and Pegna, A. J. (2017). Affective blindsight relies on low spatial frequencies. Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.009 [Epub ahead of print].
Cope, L. M., Ermer, E., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., and Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Paralimbic gray matter reductions in incarcerated adolescent females with psychopathic traits. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 659–668. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9810-4
Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Velasquez, M., Mesow, M., Feinstein, J., Canales-Johnson, A., et al. (2015). Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: a preliminary study. Auton. Neurosci. 193, 132–137. doi: 10.1016/j. autneu.2015.06.006
Davies-Thompson, J., Elli, G. V., Rezk, M., Benetti, S., van Ackeren, M., and Collignon, O. (2018). Hierarchical brain network for face and voice integration of emotion expression. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy240 [Epub ahead of print].
de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 242–249. doi: 10.1038/nrn1872
de Gelder, B., Bocker, K. B., Tuomainen, J., Hensen, M., and Vroomen, J. (1999). The combined perception of emotion from voice and face: early interaction revealed by human electric brain responses. Neurosci. Lett. 260, 133–136. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00963-x
de Gelder, B., Meeren, H. K., Righart, R., van den Stock, J., van de Riet, W. A., and Tamietto, M. (2006). Beyond the face: exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Prog. Brain Res. 155, 37–48. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55003-4
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., and Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 16701–16706. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407042101 de Pedraza, F. G., de Rincón, D. M., and Montealegre, G. (2012). Validación de la prueba de J. C. Raven: matrices progresivas y de la prueba ACE para estudiantes de primer año universitario. Rev. Colomb. Psicol. 5, 129–136. doi: 10.15446/rcp
Diano, M., Celeghin, A., Bagnis, A., and Tamietto, M. (2017). Amygdala response to emotional stimuli without awareness: facts and interpretations. Front. Psychol. 7:2029. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02029
Dodge, K. A., Price, J. M., Bachorowski, J. A., and Newman, J. P. (1990). Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 99, 385–392. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.4.385
Escobar, M. J., Huepe, D., Decety, J., Sedeño, L., Messow, M. K., Baez, S., et al. (2014). Brain signatures of moral sensitivity in adolescents with early social deprivation. Sci. Rep. 4:5354. doi: 10.1038/srep05354
Etkin, A., Egner, T., and Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004
Fairchild, G., Stobbe, Y., van Goozen, S. H., Calder, A. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2010). Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 272–279. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.019
Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Calder, A. J., Stollery, S. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolescents with earlyonset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 627–636. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02020.x
Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2008). Fear conditioning and affective modulation of the startle reflex in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and healthy control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 279–285. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007. 06.019
Ferrari, C., Lega, C., Vernice, M., Tamietto, M., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Vecchi, T., et al. (2016). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in integrating social impressions from faces and verbal descriptions. Cereb. Cortex 26, 156–165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu186
Frith, C. (2009). Role of facial expressions in social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3453–3458. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0142
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Herrera, E., Parra, M., Gomez Mendez, P., Baez, S., Manes, F., et al. (2014). Emotion recognition and cognitive empathy deficits in adolescent offenders revealed by context-sensitive tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:850. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00850
Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., and de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. Neuroimage 35, 959–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.030 Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., and Bentin, S. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emot. Rev. 5, 60–65. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451331
Hubble, K., Bowen, K. L., Moore, S. C., and van Goozen, S. H. (2015). Improving negative emotion recognition in young offenders reduces subsequent crime. PLoS One 10:e0132035. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132035
Ibáñez, A., Aguado, J., Baez, S., Huepe, D., Lopez, V., Ortega, R., et al. (2014). From neural signatures of emotional modulation to social cognition: individual differences in healthy volunteers and psychiatric participants. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 939–950. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst067
Ibañez, A., and Manes, F. (2012). Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 78, 1354–1362. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e3182518375
Jusyte, A., and Schonenberg, M. (2017). Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: perceptual deficit or cognitive bias? Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 267, 257–266. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0727-0
Kanwisher, N., and Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934
Karnath, H. O., Baier, B., and Nägele, T. (2005). Awareness of the functioning of one’s own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? J. Neurosci. 25, 7134–7138. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005
Kret, M. E., and de Gelder, B. (2013). When a smile becomes a fist: the perception of facial and bodily expressions of emotion in violent offenders. Exp. Brain Res. 228, 399–410. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3557-6
Kuin, N. C., Masthoff, E. D. M., Munafo, M. R., and Penton-Voak, I. S. (2017). Perceiving the evil eye: investigating hostile interpretation of ambiguous facial emotional expression in violent and non-violent offenders. PLoS One 12:e0187080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187080
Kumfor, F., Ibañez, A., Hutchings, R., Hazelton, J., Hodges, J., and Piguet, O. (2018). Beyond the face: how context modulates emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia subtypes. Brain 141, 1172–1185. doi: 10.1093/brain/ awy002
Lenroot, R. K., and Giedd, J. N. (2010). Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 72, 46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008
Maier, M. E., and di Pellegrino, G. (2012). Impaired conflict adaptation in an emotional task context following rostral anterior cingulate cortex lesions in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2070–2079. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00266
Maren, S., Phan, K. L., and Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492
McLeod, J. D., Uemura, R., and Rohrman, S. (2012). Adolescent mental health, behavior problems, and academic achievement. J. Health Soc. Behav. 53, 482–497. doi: 10.1177/0022146512462888
Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., de la Fuente, L., Forno, G., et al. (2016). Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 3022–3040. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww231
Neal, D. T., and Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 673–678. doi: 10.1177/1948550611406138
Nunes, D., Monteiro, L., and Lopes, E. (2014). INECO frontal screening: a tool to assess executive functions in depression. Psicol. Clin. 26, 177–196. doi: 10.1590/S0103-56652014000200011
O’Callaghan, C., Bertoux, M., Irish, M., Shine, J. M., Wong, S., Spiliopoulos, L., et al. (2016). Fair play: social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 139, 204–216. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv315
Onitsuka, T., Shenton, M. E., Kasai, K., Nestor, P. G., Toner, S. K., Kikinis, R., et al. (2003). Fusiform gyrus volume reduction and facial recognition in chronic schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 349–355. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60. 4.349
Oosterhof, N. N., and Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 11087–11092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105
Orgs, G., Dovern, A., Hagura, N., Haggard, P., Fink, G. R., and Weiss, P. H. (2016). Constructing visual perception of body movement with the motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 440–449. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv262
Passamonti, L., Fairchild, G., Goodyer, I. M., Hurford, G., Hagan, C. C.,
Rowe, J. B., et al. (2010). Neural abnormalities in early-onset and adolescenceonset conduct disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 729–738. doi: 10.1001/ archgenpsychiatry.2010.75
Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., and Vuilleumier, P. (2007). Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 274–283. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm023
Peelen, M. V., and Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 636–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn2195
Pereira, M., de Oliveira, L., Erthal, F., Joffily, M., Mocaiber, I., Volchan, E., et al. (2010). Emotion affects action: midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 94–106. doi: 10.3758/cabn.10.1.94
Philipp-Wiegmann, F., Rösler, M., Retz-Junginger, P., and Retz, W. (2017). Emotional facial recognition in proactive and reactive violent offenders. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 267, 687–695. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017- 0776-z
Piotrowska, P. J., Stride, C. B., Croft, S. E., and Rowe, R. (2015). Socioeconomic status and antisocial behaviour among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 35, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014. 11.003
Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Diamond, B., and Reingle, J. M. (2015). A systematic review of age, sex, ethnicity, and race as predictors of violent recidivism. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 59, 5–26. doi: 10.1177/ 0306624x13514733
Poyo Solanas, M., Zhan, M., Vaessen, M., Hortensius, R., Engelen, T., and de Gelder, B. (2018). Looking at the face and seeing the whole body. Neural basis of combined face and body expressions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 135–144. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx130
Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to Standard Progressive Matrices. London: HK Lewis.
Rogers, J. C., and De Brito, S. A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 64–72. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2423
Santamaría-García, H., Baez, S., Reyes, P., Santamaría-García, J. A., SantacruzEscudero, J. M., Matallana, D., et al. (2017). A lesion model of envy and Schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration. Brain 140, 3357–3377. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx269
Sato, W., Uono, S., Matsuura, N., and Toichi, M. (2009). Misrecognition of facial expressions in delinquents. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 3:27. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-3-27
Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., and Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurosci. 25, 11055–11059. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-05.2005
Seruca, T., and Silva, C. F. (2016). Executive functioning in criminal behavior: differentiating between types of crime and exploring the relation between shifting, inhibition, and anger. Int. J. Forensic Ment. Health 15, 235–246. doi: 10.1080/14999013.2016.1158755
Shahid, H., Sebastian, R., Schnur, T. T., Hanayik, T., Wright, A., Tippett, D. C., et al. (2017). Important considerations in lesion-symptom mapping: illustrations from studies of word comprehension. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 2990–3000. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23567
Sokolov, A. A., Gharabaghi, A., Tatagiba, M. S., and Pavlova, M. (2010). Cerebellar engagement in an action observation network. Cereb. Cortex 20, 486–491. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp117
Stams, G. J., Brugman, D., Dekovic, M., van Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., and Gibbs, J. C. (2006). The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: a metaanalysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 34, 697–713. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006- 9056-5
Tamietto, M., and de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn2889
Terribilli, D., Schaufelberger, M. S., Duran, F. L., Zanetti, M. V., Curiati, P. K., Menezes, P. R., et al. (2011). Age-related gray matter volume changes in the brain during non-elderly adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 354–368. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.008
Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., López, P., and Manes, F. (2009). INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 777–786. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990415
Uono, S., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., et al. (2017). Neural substrates of the ability to recognize facial expressions: a voxel-based morphometry study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 487–495. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw142
van den Bos, W., Vahl, P., Güro˘glu, B., van Nunspeet, F., Colins, O., Markus, M., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of social decision-making in severely antisocial adolescents. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 2059–2066. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu003
van de Riet, W. A., Grezes, J., and de Gelder, B. (2009). Specific and common brain regions involved in the perception of faces and bodies and the representation of their emotional expressions. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 101–120. doi: 10.1080/17470910701865367
Van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Sorger, B., Pichon, S., Grézes, J., and de Gelder, B. (2011). Cortico-subcortical visual, somatosensory, and motor activations for perceiving dynamic whole-body emotional expressions with and without striate cortex (V1). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 16188–16193. doi: 10.1073/pnas. 1107214108
Van den Stock, J., Vandenbulcke, M., Sinke, C. B., Goebel, R., and de Gelder, B. (2014). How affective information from faces and scenes interacts in the brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1481–1488. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst138
Vilà-Balló, A., Cunillera, T., Rostan, C., Hdez-Lafuente, P., Fuentemilla, L., and Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2015). Neurophysiological correlates of cognitive flexibility and feedback processing in violent juvenile offenders. Brain Res. 1610, 98–109. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.03.040
Vuilleumier, P., and Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 837–855. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2092
Vytal, K., and Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2864–2885. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21366
Wieser, M. J., and Keil, A. (2014). Fearful faces heighten the cortical representation of contextual threat. Neuroimage 86, 317–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013. 10.008
Yue, T., Pan, W., and Huang, X. (2016). The relationship between trait positive empathy and brain structure: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroreport 27, 422–426. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000557
Zhang, S., and Li, C.-S. R. (2012). Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 59, 3548–3562. doi: 10.1016/j. neuroimage.2011.11.023
Zou, Z., Meng, H., Ma, Z., Deng, W., Du, L., Wang, H., et al. (2013). Executive functioning deficits and childhood trauma in juvenile violent offenders in China. Psychiatry Res. 207, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.013
dc.rights.spa.fl_str_mv Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
dc.rights.uri.spa.fl_str_mv http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
dc.rights.accessrights.spa.fl_str_mv info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rights.coar.spa.fl_str_mv http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
rights_invalid_str_mv Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
eu_rights_str_mv openAccess
dc.publisher.spa.fl_str_mv Frontiers Media S.A.
dc.source.spa.fl_str_mv Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience
institution Corporación Universidad de la Costa
bitstream.url.fl_str_mv https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/a3e06410-f165-48e4-a2ce-2abb51dfa95b/download
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/e26e0f77-6c47-4fd7-9d04-52a78dd91a97/download
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/831767ca-54cc-4f5a-b6c4-c840eb2e656d/download
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/8eef218b-03cd-4e0a-8c07-cd3e8c2e6bfa/download
https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/987ee0c7-fcde-4f91-8136-90e41c48af9f/download
bitstream.checksum.fl_str_mv 6043b291ad1412747369942308b69866
934f4ca17e109e0a05eaeaba504d7ce4
8a4605be74aa9ea9d79846c1fba20a33
bd7756a6787adbffc5c1ec0c22fead45
d7a5cd9590c0b60c613c131683770fca
bitstream.checksumAlgorithm.fl_str_mv MD5
MD5
MD5
MD5
MD5
repository.name.fl_str_mv Repositorio de la Universidad de la Costa CUC
repository.mail.fl_str_mv repdigital@cuc.edu.co
_version_ 1811760765185556480
spelling Santamaría-García, HernandoIbáñez, AgustínMontaño, SynellaGarcía, Adolfo MartínPatiño-Saenz, MichelPino, MarianaIdarraga, ClaudiaBaez, Sandra Jimena2019-05-15T12:51:27Z2019-05-15T12:51:27Z2019-02-071662-5153https://hdl.handle.net/11323/3326Corporación Universidad de la CostaREDICUC - Repositorio CUChttps://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/Background: Adolescent offenders (AOs) are characterized by social-norm transgression and aggressive behaviors. Those traits have been associated with alterations in socio-cognitive processes, including facial emotion recognition. While this would suggest that AOs tend to interpret negative emotional cues as threatening information, most research has relied on context-free stimuli, thus failing to directly track integrative processes typical of everyday cognition. Methods: In this study, we assessed the impact of body language and surrounding context on facial emotion recognition in AOs and non-offenders (NOs). We recruited 35 AOs from a reform school for young male offenders and 30 NOs matched for age and sex with the former group. All participants completed a well-validated task aimed to determine how contextual cues (i.e., emotional body language and surrounding context) influence facial emotion recognition through the use of congruent and incongruent combinations of facial and bodily emotional information. Results: This study showed that AOs tend to overvalue bodily and contextual signals in emotion recognition, with poorer facial-emotion categorization and increased sensitivity to context information in incongruent face-body scenarios. This pattern was associated with executive dysfunctions and disruptive behaviors, as well as with gray matter (GM) of brain regions supporting body-face recognition [fusiform gyrus (FG)], emotion processing [cingulate cortex (CC), superior temporal gyrus (STG)], contextual integration (precuneus, STG), and motor resonance [cerebellum, supplementary motor area (SMA)]. Discussion: Together, our results pave the way for a better understanding of the neurocognitive association between contextual emotion recognition, behavioral regulation, cognitive control, and externalized behaviors in AOs.Antecedentes: los delincuentes adolescentes (AO) se caracterizan por la transgresión de las normas sociales y los comportamientos agresivos. Esos rasgos se han asociado con alteraciones en los procesos sociocognitivos, incluido el reconocimiento de emociones faciales. Si bien esto sugiere que los AO tienden a interpretar señales emocionales negativas como información amenazadora, la mayoría de las investigaciones se basan en estímulos sin contexto, por lo que no logran rastrear directamente los procesos de integración típicos de la cognición cotidiana. Métodos: en este estudio, evaluamos el impacto del lenguaje corporal y el contexto circundante en el reconocimiento de emociones faciales en AO y no delincuentes (NO). Reclutamos a 35 AO de una escuela de reforma para jóvenes infractores y 30 NO combinados por edad y sexo con el grupo anterior. Todos los participantes completaron una tarea bien validada dirigida a determinar cómo las señales contextuales (es decir, el lenguaje corporal emocional y el contexto circundante) influyen en el reconocimiento de la emoción facial mediante el uso de combinaciones congruentes e incongruentes de información emocional facial y corporal. Resultados: este estudio demostró que los AO tienden a sobrevalorar las señales corporales y contextuales en el reconocimiento de emociones, con una clasificación más pobre de la emoción facial y una mayor sensibilidad a la información de contexto en escenarios de rostro y cuerpo incongruentes. Este patrón se asoció con disfunciones ejecutivas y conductas disruptivas, así como con la materia gris (GM) de las regiones cerebrales que apoyan el reconocimiento cuerpo-cara [giro fusiforme (FG)], procesamiento de emociones [corteza cingulada (CC), giro temporal superior (STG) )], integración contextual (precuneus, STG) y resonancia motora [cerebelo, área motora suplementaria (SMA)]. Discusión: Juntos, nuestros resultados allanan el camino para una mejor comprensión de la asociación neurocognitiva entre el reconocimiento de la emoción contextual, la regulación del comportamiento, el control cognitivo y las conductas externalizadas en los AO.Santamaría-García, Hernando-2673bdf8-6d0b-42f9-8b0f-88affb94e029-0Ibáñez, Agustín-71ac4814-e923-4602-839b-fed937035234-0Montaño, Synella-7b270ec2-b4ac-4594-8ec8-5126b7e75370-0García, Adolfo Martín-6090a73e-80df-4281-aab9-0329924f7d5f-0Patiño-Saenz, Michel-f0b8126a-7aaa-4022-8834-01087c409ef0-0Pino, Mariana-6236b35b-bf44-4547-9eec-e9f84a20c7df-0Idarraga, Claudia-090d659b-d392-4c3e-b746-8c745931ae58-0Baez, Sandra Jimena-8a5cc083-4177-4f09-98d9-070083733895-0engFrontiers Media S.A.DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00034Adolfi, F., Couto, B., Richter, F., Decety, J., Lopez, J., Sigman, M., et al. (2016). Convergence of interoception, emotion, and social cognition: a twofold fMRI meta-analysis and lesion approach. Cortex 88, 124–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex. 2016.12.019Ahmed, S. P., Bittencourt-Hewitt, A., and Sebastian, C. L. (2015). Neurocognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.006Amoruso, L., Gelormini, C., Aboitiz, F., Alvarez Gonzalez, M., Manes, F., Cardona, J. F., et al. (2013). N400 ERPs for actions: building meaning in context. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:57. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00057Ashburner, J., and Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage 11, 805–821. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582Aviezer, H., Bentin, S., Hassin, R. R., Meschino, W. S., Kennedy, J., Grewal, S., et al. (2009). Not on the face alone: perception of contextualized face expressions in Huntington’s disease. Brain 132, 1633–1644. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp067Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., et al. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or Afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychol. Sci. 19, 724–732. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02148.xAviezer, H., Trope, Y., and Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science 338, 1225–1229. doi: 10.1126/science.1224313Bachmann, J., Munzert, J., and Krüger, B. (2018). Neural underpinnings of the perception of emotional states derived from biological human motion: a review of neuroimaging research. Front. Psychol. 9:1763. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01763Baez, S., García, A. M., and Ibanez, A. (2017a). The social context network model in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 30, 379–396. doi: 10.1007/7854_2016_443Baez, S., Herrera, E., García, A., Manes, F., Young, L., and Ibáñez, A. (2017b). Outcome-oriented moral evaluation in terrorists. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0165. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0165Baez, S., Herrera, E., García, A. M., Huepe, D., Santamaría-García, H., and Ibáñez, A. (2018a). Increased moral condemnation of accidental harm in institutionalized adolescents. Sci. Rep. 8:11609. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29956-9Baez, S., Pino, M., Berrío, M., Santamaría-García, H., Sedeño, L., García, A. M., et al. (2018b). Corticostriatal signatures of schadenfreude: evidence from Huntington’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 89, 112–116. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-316055Baez, S., and Ibanez, A. (2014). The effects of context processing on social cognition impairments in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Front. Neurosci. 8:270. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00270Baez, S., Ibanez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., Perez, A., Roca, M., Manes, F., et al. (2014). The utility of IFS (INECO Frontal Screening) for the detection of executive dysfunction in adults with bipolar disorder and ADHD. Psychiatry Res. 216, 269–276. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.020Baez, S., Rattazzi, A., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Torralva, T., Vigliecca, N. S., Decety, J., et al. (2012). Integrating intention and context: assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:302. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00302Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., and Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 286–290. doi: 10.1177/09637214114 22522Best, M., Williams, J. M., and Coccaro, E. F. (2002). Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 8448–8453. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1126 04099Blair, R. J., Colledge, E., Murray, L., and Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 491–498. doi: 10.1023/A:1012225108281Bowen, K. L., Morgan, J. E., Moore, S. C., and van Goozen, S. H. M. (2014). Young offenders’ emotion recognition dysfunction across emotion intensities: explaining variation using psychopathic traits, conduct disorder and offense severity. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 36, 60–73. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013- 9368-zBrazil, I. A., Maes, J. H., Scheper, I., Bulten, B. H., Kessels, R. P., Verkes, R. J., et al. (2013). Reversal deficits in individuals with psychopathy in explici but not implicit learning conditions. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 38, E13–E20. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120152Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., and Schweitzer, J. (2009). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics 123, 1472–1476. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1406Budhiraja, M., Savic, I., Lindner, P., Jokinen, J., Tiihonen, J., and Hodgins, S. (2017). Brain structure abnormalities in young women who presented conduct disorder in childhood/adolescence. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 869–885. doi: 10.3758/s13415-017-0519-7Burnett, S., Sebastian, C., Cohen Kadosh, K., and Blakemore, S.-J. (2011). The social brain in adolescence: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1654–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.011Burra, N., Hervais-Adelman, A., Celeghin, A., de Gelder, B., and Pegna, A. J. (2017). Affective blindsight relies on low spatial frequencies. Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.009 [Epub ahead of print].Cope, L. M., Ermer, E., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., and Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Paralimbic gray matter reductions in incarcerated adolescent females with psychopathic traits. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 659–668. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9810-4Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Velasquez, M., Mesow, M., Feinstein, J., Canales-Johnson, A., et al. (2015). Heart evoked potential triggers brain responses to natural affective scenes: a preliminary study. Auton. Neurosci. 193, 132–137. doi: 10.1016/j. autneu.2015.06.006Davies-Thompson, J., Elli, G. V., Rezk, M., Benetti, S., van Ackeren, M., and Collignon, O. (2018). Hierarchical brain network for face and voice integration of emotion expression. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy240 [Epub ahead of print].de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 242–249. doi: 10.1038/nrn1872de Gelder, B., Bocker, K. B., Tuomainen, J., Hensen, M., and Vroomen, J. (1999). The combined perception of emotion from voice and face: early interaction revealed by human electric brain responses. Neurosci. Lett. 260, 133–136. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00963-xde Gelder, B., Meeren, H. K., Righart, R., van den Stock, J., van de Riet, W. A., and Tamietto, M. (2006). Beyond the face: exploring rapid influences of context on face processing. Prog. Brain Res. 155, 37–48. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55003-4de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., and Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 16701–16706. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407042101 de Pedraza, F. G., de Rincón, D. M., and Montealegre, G. (2012). Validación de la prueba de J. C. Raven: matrices progresivas y de la prueba ACE para estudiantes de primer año universitario. Rev. Colomb. Psicol. 5, 129–136. doi: 10.15446/rcpDiano, M., Celeghin, A., Bagnis, A., and Tamietto, M. (2017). Amygdala response to emotional stimuli without awareness: facts and interpretations. Front. Psychol. 7:2029. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02029Dodge, K. A., Price, J. M., Bachorowski, J. A., and Newman, J. P. (1990). Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 99, 385–392. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.4.385Escobar, M. J., Huepe, D., Decety, J., Sedeño, L., Messow, M. K., Baez, S., et al. (2014). Brain signatures of moral sensitivity in adolescents with early social deprivation. Sci. Rep. 4:5354. doi: 10.1038/srep05354Etkin, A., Egner, T., and Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004Fairchild, G., Stobbe, Y., van Goozen, S. H., Calder, A. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2010). Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 272–279. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.019Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Calder, A. J., Stollery, S. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolescents with earlyonset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 627–636. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02020.xFairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H., Stollery, S. J., and Goodyer, I. M. (2008). Fear conditioning and affective modulation of the startle reflex in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and healthy control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 279–285. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007. 06.019Ferrari, C., Lega, C., Vernice, M., Tamietto, M., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Vecchi, T., et al. (2016). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex plays a causal role in integrating social impressions from faces and verbal descriptions. Cereb. Cortex 26, 156–165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu186Frith, C. (2009). Role of facial expressions in social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3453–3458. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0142Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Herrera, E., Parra, M., Gomez Mendez, P., Baez, S., Manes, F., et al. (2014). Emotion recognition and cognitive empathy deficits in adolescent offenders revealed by context-sensitive tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:850. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00850Grèzes, J., Pichon, S., and de Gelder, B. (2007). Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions. Neuroimage 35, 959–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.030 Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., and Bentin, S. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emot. Rev. 5, 60–65. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451331Hubble, K., Bowen, K. L., Moore, S. C., and van Goozen, S. H. (2015). Improving negative emotion recognition in young offenders reduces subsequent crime. PLoS One 10:e0132035. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132035Ibáñez, A., Aguado, J., Baez, S., Huepe, D., Lopez, V., Ortega, R., et al. (2014). From neural signatures of emotional modulation to social cognition: individual differences in healthy volunteers and psychiatric participants. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 939–950. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst067Ibañez, A., and Manes, F. (2012). Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 78, 1354–1362. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0b013e3182518375Jusyte, A., and Schonenberg, M. (2017). Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: perceptual deficit or cognitive bias? Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 267, 257–266. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0727-0Kanwisher, N., and Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934Karnath, H. O., Baier, B., and Nägele, T. (2005). Awareness of the functioning of one’s own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? J. Neurosci. 25, 7134–7138. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005Kret, M. E., and de Gelder, B. (2013). When a smile becomes a fist: the perception of facial and bodily expressions of emotion in violent offenders. Exp. Brain Res. 228, 399–410. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3557-6Kuin, N. C., Masthoff, E. D. M., Munafo, M. R., and Penton-Voak, I. S. (2017). Perceiving the evil eye: investigating hostile interpretation of ambiguous facial emotional expression in violent and non-violent offenders. PLoS One 12:e0187080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187080Kumfor, F., Ibañez, A., Hutchings, R., Hazelton, J., Hodges, J., and Piguet, O. (2018). Beyond the face: how context modulates emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia subtypes. Brain 141, 1172–1185. doi: 10.1093/brain/ awy002Lenroot, R. K., and Giedd, J. N. (2010). Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 72, 46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008Maier, M. E., and di Pellegrino, G. (2012). Impaired conflict adaptation in an emotional task context following rostral anterior cingulate cortex lesions in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2070–2079. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00266Maren, S., Phan, K. L., and Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492McLeod, J. D., Uemura, R., and Rohrman, S. (2012). Adolescent mental health, behavior problems, and academic achievement. J. Health Soc. Behav. 53, 482–497. doi: 10.1177/0022146512462888Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., de la Fuente, L., Forno, G., et al. (2016). Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 3022–3040. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww231Neal, D. T., and Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 673–678. doi: 10.1177/1948550611406138Nunes, D., Monteiro, L., and Lopes, E. (2014). INECO frontal screening: a tool to assess executive functions in depression. Psicol. Clin. 26, 177–196. doi: 10.1590/S0103-56652014000200011O’Callaghan, C., Bertoux, M., Irish, M., Shine, J. M., Wong, S., Spiliopoulos, L., et al. (2016). Fair play: social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 139, 204–216. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv315Onitsuka, T., Shenton, M. E., Kasai, K., Nestor, P. G., Toner, S. K., Kikinis, R., et al. (2003). Fusiform gyrus volume reduction and facial recognition in chronic schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 349–355. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60. 4.349Oosterhof, N. N., and Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 11087–11092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105Orgs, G., Dovern, A., Hagura, N., Haggard, P., Fink, G. R., and Weiss, P. H. (2016). Constructing visual perception of body movement with the motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex 26, 440–449. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv262Passamonti, L., Fairchild, G., Goodyer, I. M., Hurford, G., Hagan, C. C.,Rowe, J. B., et al. (2010). Neural abnormalities in early-onset and adolescenceonset conduct disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 729–738. doi: 10.1001/ archgenpsychiatry.2010.75Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., and Vuilleumier, P. (2007). Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 274–283. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm023Peelen, M. V., and Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 636–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn2195Pereira, M., de Oliveira, L., Erthal, F., Joffily, M., Mocaiber, I., Volchan, E., et al. (2010). Emotion affects action: midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 94–106. doi: 10.3758/cabn.10.1.94Philipp-Wiegmann, F., Rösler, M., Retz-Junginger, P., and Retz, W. (2017). Emotional facial recognition in proactive and reactive violent offenders. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 267, 687–695. doi: 10.1007/s00406-017- 0776-zPiotrowska, P. J., Stride, C. B., Croft, S. E., and Rowe, R. (2015). Socioeconomic status and antisocial behaviour among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 35, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014. 11.003Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Diamond, B., and Reingle, J. M. (2015). A systematic review of age, sex, ethnicity, and race as predictors of violent recidivism. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 59, 5–26. doi: 10.1177/ 0306624x13514733Poyo Solanas, M., Zhan, M., Vaessen, M., Hortensius, R., Engelen, T., and de Gelder, B. (2018). Looking at the face and seeing the whole body. Neural basis of combined face and body expressions. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 135–144. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx130Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to Standard Progressive Matrices. London: HK Lewis.Rogers, J. C., and De Brito, S. A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 64–72. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2423Santamaría-García, H., Baez, S., Reyes, P., Santamaría-García, J. A., SantacruzEscudero, J. M., Matallana, D., et al. (2017). A lesion model of envy and Schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration. Brain 140, 3357–3377. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx269Sato, W., Uono, S., Matsuura, N., and Toichi, M. (2009). Misrecognition of facial expressions in delinquents. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 3:27. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-3-27Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., and Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurosci. 25, 11055–11059. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-05.2005Seruca, T., and Silva, C. F. (2016). Executive functioning in criminal behavior: differentiating between types of crime and exploring the relation between shifting, inhibition, and anger. Int. J. Forensic Ment. Health 15, 235–246. doi: 10.1080/14999013.2016.1158755Shahid, H., Sebastian, R., Schnur, T. T., Hanayik, T., Wright, A., Tippett, D. C., et al. (2017). Important considerations in lesion-symptom mapping: illustrations from studies of word comprehension. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 2990–3000. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23567Sokolov, A. A., Gharabaghi, A., Tatagiba, M. S., and Pavlova, M. (2010). Cerebellar engagement in an action observation network. Cereb. Cortex 20, 486–491. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp117Stams, G. J., Brugman, D., Dekovic, M., van Rosmalen, L., van der Laan, P., and Gibbs, J. C. (2006). The moral judgment of juvenile delinquents: a metaanalysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 34, 697–713. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006- 9056-5Tamietto, M., and de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn2889Terribilli, D., Schaufelberger, M. S., Duran, F. L., Zanetti, M. V., Curiati, P. K., Menezes, P. R., et al. (2011). Age-related gray matter volume changes in the brain during non-elderly adulthood. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 354–368. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.008Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., López, P., and Manes, F. (2009). INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 777–786. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990415Uono, S., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Sawada, R., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., et al. (2017). Neural substrates of the ability to recognize facial expressions: a voxel-based morphometry study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 487–495. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw142van den Bos, W., Vahl, P., Güro˘glu, B., van Nunspeet, F., Colins, O., Markus, M., et al. (2014). Neural correlates of social decision-making in severely antisocial adolescents. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 2059–2066. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu003van de Riet, W. A., Grezes, J., and de Gelder, B. (2009). Specific and common brain regions involved in the perception of faces and bodies and the representation of their emotional expressions. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 101–120. doi: 10.1080/17470910701865367Van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Sorger, B., Pichon, S., Grézes, J., and de Gelder, B. (2011). Cortico-subcortical visual, somatosensory, and motor activations for perceiving dynamic whole-body emotional expressions with and without striate cortex (V1). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 16188–16193. doi: 10.1073/pnas. 1107214108Van den Stock, J., Vandenbulcke, M., Sinke, C. B., Goebel, R., and de Gelder, B. (2014). How affective information from faces and scenes interacts in the brain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1481–1488. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst138Vilà-Balló, A., Cunillera, T., Rostan, C., Hdez-Lafuente, P., Fuentemilla, L., and Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2015). Neurophysiological correlates of cognitive flexibility and feedback processing in violent juvenile offenders. Brain Res. 1610, 98–109. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.03.040Vuilleumier, P., and Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 837–855. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2092Vytal, K., and Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2864–2885. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21366Wieser, M. J., and Keil, A. (2014). Fearful faces heighten the cortical representation of contextual threat. Neuroimage 86, 317–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013. 10.008Yue, T., Pan, W., and Huang, X. (2016). The relationship between trait positive empathy and brain structure: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroreport 27, 422–426. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000557Zhang, S., and Li, C.-S. R. (2012). Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 59, 3548–3562. doi: 10.1016/j. neuroimage.2011.11.023Zou, Z., Meng, H., Ma, Z., Deng, W., Du, L., Wang, H., et al. (2013). Executive functioning deficits and childhood trauma in juvenile violent offenders in China. Psychiatry Res. 207, 218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.09.013Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 Internationalhttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2Frontiers in behavioral neuroscienceAdolescent offendersEmotion recognitionEmotion integrationBrain morphologyDisruptive behaviorsDelincuentes adolescentesReconocimiento de emocionesIntegración de emocionesMorfología cerebralConductas disruptivasOut of context, beyond the face: Neuroanatomical pathways of emotional face-body language integration in adolescent offendersFuera de contexto, más allá de la cara: vías neuroanatómicas de la integración emocional del lenguaje cara-cuerpo en adolescentes infractoresArtículo de revistahttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1Textinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlehttp://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ARTinfo:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersionPublicationORIGINALOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdfOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdfapplication/pdf2093095https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/a3e06410-f165-48e4-a2ce-2abb51dfa95b/download6043b291ad1412747369942308b69866MD51CC-LICENSElicense_rdflicense_rdfapplication/rdf+xml; charset=utf-81031https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/e26e0f77-6c47-4fd7-9d04-52a78dd91a97/download934f4ca17e109e0a05eaeaba504d7ce4MD52LICENSElicense.txtlicense.txttext/plain; charset=utf-81748https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/831767ca-54cc-4f5a-b6c4-c840eb2e656d/download8a4605be74aa9ea9d79846c1fba20a33MD53THUMBNAILOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdf.jpgOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdf.jpgimage/jpeg61006https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/8eef218b-03cd-4e0a-8c07-cd3e8c2e6bfa/downloadbd7756a6787adbffc5c1ec0c22fead45MD55TEXTOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdf.txtOut of Context, Beyond the Face.pdf.txttext/plain86860https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/bitstreams/987ee0c7-fcde-4f91-8136-90e41c48af9f/downloadd7a5cd9590c0b60c613c131683770fcaMD5611323/3326oai:repositorio.cuc.edu.co:11323/33262024-09-17 11:02:54.808http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 Internationalopen.accesshttps://repositorio.cuc.edu.coRepositorio de la Universidad de la Costa CUCrepdigital@cuc.edu.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